Library Home

Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction

(0 reviews)

Valerie Sheppard, JIBC

Copyright Year: 2020

Last Update: 2024

Publisher: BCcampus

Language: English

Formats Available

Conditions of use.

Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike

Table of Contents

  • Accessibility Statement
  • About This Book
  • Chapter 1: Introduction to Research Methods
  • Chapter 2: Ethics in Research
  • Chapter 3: Developing a Research Question
  • Chapter 4: Measurement and Units of Analysis
  • Chapter 5: The Literature Review
  • Chapter 6: Data Collection Strategies
  • Chapter 7: Sampling Techniques
  • Chapter 8: Data Collection Methods: Survey Research
  • Chapter 9: Analysis Of Survey Data
  • Chapter 10: Qualitative Data Collection & Analysis Methods
  • Chapter 11: Quantitative Interview Techniques & Considerations
  • Chapter 12: Field Research: A Qualitative Research Technique
  • Chapter 13: Unobtrusive Research: Qualitative And Quantitative Approaches
  • Chapter 14: The Research Proposal
  • Chapter 15: Sharing Your Research
  • Chapter 16: Reading and Understanding Social Research
  • Chapter 17: Research Methods in the Real World
  • List of Links

Ancillary Material

About the book.

This textbook provides a broad overview of research methods utilized in sociology. It will be of particular value for students who are new to research methods.

About the Contributors

Valerie Sheppard , JIBC

Contribute to this Page

Research Methods in the Social Sciences: An A-Z of key concepts

Research Methods in the Social Sciences features chapters that cover a wide range of concepts, methods, and theories. Each chapter begins with an introduction to a method, using real-world examples from a wide range of academic disciplines, before discussing the benefits and limitations of the approach, its current status in academic practice, and finally providing tips and advice on when and how to apply the method in research. The text covers both well-established concepts and emerging ideas, such as big data and network analysis, for qualitative and quantitative research methods.

  • Related Documents

Stirring the Frequentist Pot with a Dash of Bayes

Rethinking Social Inquiry (RSI) is a key turning point in a long arc of development and contestation within and between qualitative and quantitative research methods in the social sciences. It builds on and further advances three important trends in these research methods: a renaissance in qualitative methods in the last decade, the continuing refinement of statistical and formal methods, and a nascent convergence of methodologists of all kinds behind a more pluralistic vision of methodology that includes growing interest in multimethod work. RSI achieves these contributions not just substantively but symbolically, bringing together leading methodologists in the quantitative and qualitative traditions, most notably the editors themselves, to address the tough issue of what would constitute shared standards for good research regardless of method. Although much of the initial commentary on RSI will no doubt focus on its critiques of Designing Social Inquiry, I suspect that in the long run the subtitle of RSI (“Diverse Tools, Shared Standards”) better captures what will be its lasting contribution to the social sciences.

Quantitative research: Methods in the social sciences

A tale of two cultures.

Some in the social sciences argue that the same logic applies to both qualitative and quantitative research methods. This book demonstrates that these two paradigms constitute different cultures, each internally coherent yet marked by contrasting norms, practices, and toolkits. The book identifies and discusses major differences between these two traditions that touch nearly every aspect of social science research, including design, goals, causal effects and models, concepts and measurement, data analysis, and case selection. Although focused on the differences between qualitative and quantitative research, the book also seeks to promote toleration, exchange, and learning by enabling scholars to think beyond their own culture and see an alternative scientific worldview. The book is written in an easily accessible style and features a host of real-world examples to illustrate methodological points.

The origins, development, and application of Qualitative Comparative Analysis: the first 25 years

A quarter century ago, in 1987, Charles C. Ragin published The Comparative Method, introducing a new method to the social sciences called Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA). QCA is a comparative case-oriented research approach and collection of techniques based on set theory and Boolean algebra, which aims to combine some of the strengths of qualitative and quantitative research methods. Since its launch in 1987, QCA has been applied extensively in the social sciences. This review essay first sketches the origins of the ideas behind QCA. Next, the main features of the method, as presented in The Comparative Method, are introduced. A third part focuses on the early applications. A fourth part presents early criticisms and subsequent innovations. A fifth part then focuses on an era of further expansion in political science and presents some of the main applications in the discipline. In doing so, this paper seeks to provide insights and references into the origin and development of QCA, a non-technical introduction to its main features, the path travelled so far, and the diversification of applications.

Quantitative Research: Methods in the Social Sciences

Forskningstraditioner krydser deres spor - kvalitative og kvantitative socio-kulturelle empiriske forskningsmetoder.

The article takes up the discussion about qualitative and quantitative research methods as ostensibly incopatible approaches to empirical studies - an understanding which is broadly disseminated within a range of academic disciplines. The authors trouble this dualistic understanding through concrete discussions of methodological approaches conducted by as well qualitatively as quantitatively oriented reseachers. It is argued, that difference and potential incompatibility must be seen in relation to the metatheoretical basis for the studies and thereby in relation to the research ambitions, in which the studies are involved.

A Tale of Two Cultures: Qualitative and Quantitative Research in the Social Sciences. By Gary Goertz and James Mahoney. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2012. 248p. $65.00 cloth, $29.95 paper.

Qualitative and quantitative research methods: old wine in new bottles on understanding and interpreting educational phenomena, evaluating new treatments in psychiatry: the potential value of combining qualitative and quantitative research methods, analysis regarding the importance of promotion in steel companies in indonesia.

ABSTRACT  The purpose of this research is to find out the promotion and marketing strategy for steel companies in Indonesia. The research method used in this research is qualitative and quantitative research methods. Qualitative research methods include interviews with resource persons and product users (extreme users and expert users) as well as literature studies of journals related to light steel, promotion, and marketing strategy. The conclusion of this research is the need for a promotion that can be accepted by customers and to be able to promote steel companies in Indonesia.   Keyword: branding, marketing, promotion, customers, steel.

Export Citation Format

Share document.

research methods for social sciences

  • USC Libraries
  • Research Guides

Organizing Your Social Sciences Research Paper

  • 6. The Methodology
  • Purpose of Guide
  • Design Flaws to Avoid
  • Independent and Dependent Variables
  • Glossary of Research Terms
  • Reading Research Effectively
  • Narrowing a Topic Idea
  • Broadening a Topic Idea
  • Extending the Timeliness of a Topic Idea
  • Academic Writing Style
  • Applying Critical Thinking
  • Choosing a Title
  • Making an Outline
  • Paragraph Development
  • Research Process Video Series
  • Executive Summary
  • The C.A.R.S. Model
  • Background Information
  • The Research Problem/Question
  • Theoretical Framework
  • Citation Tracking
  • Content Alert Services
  • Evaluating Sources
  • Primary Sources
  • Secondary Sources
  • Tiertiary Sources
  • Scholarly vs. Popular Publications
  • Qualitative Methods
  • Quantitative Methods
  • Insiderness
  • Using Non-Textual Elements
  • Limitations of the Study
  • Common Grammar Mistakes
  • Writing Concisely
  • Avoiding Plagiarism
  • Footnotes or Endnotes?
  • Further Readings
  • Generative AI and Writing
  • USC Libraries Tutorials and Other Guides
  • Bibliography

The methods section describes actions taken to investigate a research problem and the rationale for the application of specific procedures or techniques used to identify, select, process, and analyze information applied to understanding the problem, thereby, allowing the reader to critically evaluate a study’s overall validity and reliability. The methodology section of a research paper answers two main questions: How was the data collected or generated? And, how was it analyzed? The writing should be direct and precise and always written in the past tense.

Kallet, Richard H. "How to Write the Methods Section of a Research Paper." Respiratory Care 49 (October 2004): 1229-1232.

Importance of a Good Methodology Section

You must explain how you obtained and analyzed your results for the following reasons:

  • Readers need to know how the data was obtained because the method you chose affects the results and, by extension, how you interpreted their significance in the discussion section of your paper.
  • Methodology is crucial for any branch of scholarship because an unreliable method produces unreliable results and, as a consequence, undermines the value of your analysis of the findings.
  • In most cases, there are a variety of different methods you can choose to investigate a research problem. The methodology section of your paper should clearly articulate the reasons why you have chosen a particular procedure or technique.
  • The reader wants to know that the data was collected or generated in a way that is consistent with accepted practice in the field of study. For example, if you are using a multiple choice questionnaire, readers need to know that it offered your respondents a reasonable range of answers to choose from.
  • The method must be appropriate to fulfilling the overall aims of the study. For example, you need to ensure that you have a large enough sample size to be able to generalize and make recommendations based upon the findings.
  • The methodology should discuss the problems that were anticipated and the steps you took to prevent them from occurring. For any problems that do arise, you must describe the ways in which they were minimized or why these problems do not impact in any meaningful way your interpretation of the findings.
  • In the social and behavioral sciences, it is important to always provide sufficient information to allow other researchers to adopt or replicate your methodology. This information is particularly important when a new method has been developed or an innovative use of an existing method is utilized.

Bem, Daryl J. Writing the Empirical Journal Article. Psychology Writing Center. University of Washington; Denscombe, Martyn. The Good Research Guide: For Small-Scale Social Research Projects . 5th edition. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press, 2014; Lunenburg, Frederick C. Writing a Successful Thesis or Dissertation: Tips and Strategies for Students in the Social and Behavioral Sciences . Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press, 2008.

Structure and Writing Style

I.  Groups of Research Methods

There are two main groups of research methods in the social sciences:

  • The e mpirical-analytical group approaches the study of social sciences in a similar manner that researchers study the natural sciences . This type of research focuses on objective knowledge, research questions that can be answered yes or no, and operational definitions of variables to be measured. The empirical-analytical group employs deductive reasoning that uses existing theory as a foundation for formulating hypotheses that need to be tested. This approach is focused on explanation.
  • The i nterpretative group of methods is focused on understanding phenomenon in a comprehensive, holistic way . Interpretive methods focus on analytically disclosing the meaning-making practices of human subjects [the why, how, or by what means people do what they do], while showing how those practices arrange so that it can be used to generate observable outcomes. Interpretive methods allow you to recognize your connection to the phenomena under investigation. However, the interpretative group requires careful examination of variables because it focuses more on subjective knowledge.

II.  Content

The introduction to your methodology section should begin by restating the research problem and underlying assumptions underpinning your study. This is followed by situating the methods you used to gather, analyze, and process information within the overall “tradition” of your field of study and within the particular research design you have chosen to study the problem. If the method you choose lies outside of the tradition of your field [i.e., your review of the literature demonstrates that the method is not commonly used], provide a justification for how your choice of methods specifically addresses the research problem in ways that have not been utilized in prior studies.

The remainder of your methodology section should describe the following:

  • Decisions made in selecting the data you have analyzed or, in the case of qualitative research, the subjects and research setting you have examined,
  • Tools and methods used to identify and collect information, and how you identified relevant variables,
  • The ways in which you processed the data and the procedures you used to analyze that data, and
  • The specific research tools or strategies that you utilized to study the underlying hypothesis and research questions.

In addition, an effectively written methodology section should:

  • Introduce the overall methodological approach for investigating your research problem . Is your study qualitative or quantitative or a combination of both (mixed method)? Are you going to take a special approach, such as action research, or a more neutral stance?
  • Indicate how the approach fits the overall research design . Your methods for gathering data should have a clear connection to your research problem. In other words, make sure that your methods will actually address the problem. One of the most common deficiencies found in research papers is that the proposed methodology is not suitable to achieving the stated objective of your paper.
  • Describe the specific methods of data collection you are going to use , such as, surveys, interviews, questionnaires, observation, archival research. If you are analyzing existing data, such as a data set or archival documents, describe how it was originally created or gathered and by whom. Also be sure to explain how older data is still relevant to investigating the current research problem.
  • Explain how you intend to analyze your results . Will you use statistical analysis? Will you use specific theoretical perspectives to help you analyze a text or explain observed behaviors? Describe how you plan to obtain an accurate assessment of relationships, patterns, trends, distributions, and possible contradictions found in the data.
  • Provide background and a rationale for methodologies that are unfamiliar for your readers . Very often in the social sciences, research problems and the methods for investigating them require more explanation/rationale than widely accepted rules governing the natural and physical sciences. Be clear and concise in your explanation.
  • Provide a justification for subject selection and sampling procedure . For instance, if you propose to conduct interviews, how do you intend to select the sample population? If you are analyzing texts, which texts have you chosen, and why? If you are using statistics, why is this set of data being used? If other data sources exist, explain why the data you chose is most appropriate to addressing the research problem.
  • Provide a justification for case study selection . A common method of analyzing research problems in the social sciences is to analyze specific cases. These can be a person, place, event, phenomenon, or other type of subject of analysis that are either examined as a singular topic of in-depth investigation or multiple topics of investigation studied for the purpose of comparing or contrasting findings. In either method, you should explain why a case or cases were chosen and how they specifically relate to the research problem.
  • Describe potential limitations . Are there any practical limitations that could affect your data collection? How will you attempt to control for potential confounding variables and errors? If your methodology may lead to problems you can anticipate, state this openly and show why pursuing this methodology outweighs the risk of these problems cropping up.

NOTE:   Once you have written all of the elements of the methods section, subsequent revisions should focus on how to present those elements as clearly and as logically as possibly. The description of how you prepared to study the research problem, how you gathered the data, and the protocol for analyzing the data should be organized chronologically. For clarity, when a large amount of detail must be presented, information should be presented in sub-sections according to topic. If necessary, consider using appendices for raw data.

ANOTHER NOTE: If you are conducting a qualitative analysis of a research problem , the methodology section generally requires a more elaborate description of the methods used as well as an explanation of the processes applied to gathering and analyzing of data than is generally required for studies using quantitative methods. Because you are the primary instrument for generating the data [e.g., through interviews or observations], the process for collecting that data has a significantly greater impact on producing the findings. Therefore, qualitative research requires a more detailed description of the methods used.

YET ANOTHER NOTE:   If your study involves interviews, observations, or other qualitative techniques involving human subjects , you may be required to obtain approval from the university's Office for the Protection of Research Subjects before beginning your research. This is not a common procedure for most undergraduate level student research assignments. However, i f your professor states you need approval, you must include a statement in your methods section that you received official endorsement and adequate informed consent from the office and that there was a clear assessment and minimization of risks to participants and to the university. This statement informs the reader that your study was conducted in an ethical and responsible manner. In some cases, the approval notice is included as an appendix to your paper.

III.  Problems to Avoid

Irrelevant Detail The methodology section of your paper should be thorough but concise. Do not provide any background information that does not directly help the reader understand why a particular method was chosen, how the data was gathered or obtained, and how the data was analyzed in relation to the research problem [note: analyzed, not interpreted! Save how you interpreted the findings for the discussion section]. With this in mind, the page length of your methods section will generally be less than any other section of your paper except the conclusion.

Unnecessary Explanation of Basic Procedures Remember that you are not writing a how-to guide about a particular method. You should make the assumption that readers possess a basic understanding of how to investigate the research problem on their own and, therefore, you do not have to go into great detail about specific methodological procedures. The focus should be on how you applied a method , not on the mechanics of doing a method. An exception to this rule is if you select an unconventional methodological approach; if this is the case, be sure to explain why this approach was chosen and how it enhances the overall process of discovery.

Problem Blindness It is almost a given that you will encounter problems when collecting or generating your data, or, gaps will exist in existing data or archival materials. Do not ignore these problems or pretend they did not occur. Often, documenting how you overcame obstacles can form an interesting part of the methodology. It demonstrates to the reader that you can provide a cogent rationale for the decisions you made to minimize the impact of any problems that arose.

Literature Review Just as the literature review section of your paper provides an overview of sources you have examined while researching a particular topic, the methodology section should cite any sources that informed your choice and application of a particular method [i.e., the choice of a survey should include any citations to the works you used to help construct the survey].

It’s More than Sources of Information! A description of a research study's method should not be confused with a description of the sources of information. Such a list of sources is useful in and of itself, especially if it is accompanied by an explanation about the selection and use of the sources. The description of the project's methodology complements a list of sources in that it sets forth the organization and interpretation of information emanating from those sources.

Azevedo, L.F. et al. "How to Write a Scientific Paper: Writing the Methods Section." Revista Portuguesa de Pneumologia 17 (2011): 232-238; Blair Lorrie. “Choosing a Methodology.” In Writing a Graduate Thesis or Dissertation , Teaching Writing Series. (Rotterdam: Sense Publishers 2016), pp. 49-72; Butin, Dan W. The Education Dissertation A Guide for Practitioner Scholars . Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin, 2010; Carter, Susan. Structuring Your Research Thesis . New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2012; Kallet, Richard H. “How to Write the Methods Section of a Research Paper.” Respiratory Care 49 (October 2004):1229-1232; Lunenburg, Frederick C. Writing a Successful Thesis or Dissertation: Tips and Strategies for Students in the Social and Behavioral Sciences . Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press, 2008. Methods Section. The Writer’s Handbook. Writing Center. University of Wisconsin, Madison; Rudestam, Kjell Erik and Rae R. Newton. “The Method Chapter: Describing Your Research Plan.” In Surviving Your Dissertation: A Comprehensive Guide to Content and Process . (Thousand Oaks, Sage Publications, 2015), pp. 87-115; What is Interpretive Research. Institute of Public and International Affairs, University of Utah; Writing the Experimental Report: Methods, Results, and Discussion. The Writing Lab and The OWL. Purdue University; Methods and Materials. The Structure, Format, Content, and Style of a Journal-Style Scientific Paper. Department of Biology. Bates College.

Writing Tip

Statistical Designs and Tests? Do Not Fear Them!

Don't avoid using a quantitative approach to analyzing your research problem just because you fear the idea of applying statistical designs and tests. A qualitative approach, such as conducting interviews or content analysis of archival texts, can yield exciting new insights about a research problem, but it should not be undertaken simply because you have a disdain for running a simple regression. A well designed quantitative research study can often be accomplished in very clear and direct ways, whereas, a similar study of a qualitative nature usually requires considerable time to analyze large volumes of data and a tremendous burden to create new paths for analysis where previously no path associated with your research problem had existed.

To locate data and statistics, GO HERE .

Another Writing Tip

Knowing the Relationship Between Theories and Methods

There can be multiple meaning associated with the term "theories" and the term "methods" in social sciences research. A helpful way to delineate between them is to understand "theories" as representing different ways of characterizing the social world when you research it and "methods" as representing different ways of generating and analyzing data about that social world. Framed in this way, all empirical social sciences research involves theories and methods, whether they are stated explicitly or not. However, while theories and methods are often related, it is important that, as a researcher, you deliberately separate them in order to avoid your theories playing a disproportionate role in shaping what outcomes your chosen methods produce.

Introspectively engage in an ongoing dialectic between the application of theories and methods to help enable you to use the outcomes from your methods to interrogate and develop new theories, or ways of framing conceptually the research problem. This is how scholarship grows and branches out into new intellectual territory.

Reynolds, R. Larry. Ways of Knowing. Alternative Microeconomics . Part 1, Chapter 3. Boise State University; The Theory-Method Relationship. S-Cool Revision. United Kingdom.

Yet Another Writing Tip

Methods and the Methodology

Do not confuse the terms "methods" and "methodology." As Schneider notes, a method refers to the technical steps taken to do research . Descriptions of methods usually include defining and stating why you have chosen specific techniques to investigate a research problem, followed by an outline of the procedures you used to systematically select, gather, and process the data [remember to always save the interpretation of data for the discussion section of your paper].

The methodology refers to a discussion of the underlying reasoning why particular methods were used . This discussion includes describing the theoretical concepts that inform the choice of methods to be applied, placing the choice of methods within the more general nature of academic work, and reviewing its relevance to examining the research problem. The methodology section also includes a thorough review of the methods other scholars have used to study the topic.

Bryman, Alan. "Of Methods and Methodology." Qualitative Research in Organizations and Management: An International Journal 3 (2008): 159-168; Schneider, Florian. “What's in a Methodology: The Difference between Method, Methodology, and Theory…and How to Get the Balance Right?” PoliticsEastAsia.com. Chinese Department, University of Leiden, Netherlands.

  • << Previous: Scholarly vs. Popular Publications
  • Next: Qualitative Methods >>
  • Last Updated: Jul 30, 2024 10:20 AM
  • URL: https://libguides.usc.edu/writingguide

Research Methods for the Social Sciences

The content in this course was curated by Professor Bill Pelz at Herkimer College / SUNY and is provided under a Creative Commons license.

book-cover

Table of Contents

  • Chapter 1 Science and Scientific Research
  • Chapter 2 Thinking Like a Researcher
  • Chapter 3 The Research Process
  • Chapter 4 Theories in Scientific Research
  • Chapter 5 Research Design
  • Chapter 6 Measurement of Constructs
  • Chapter 7 Scale Reliability and Validity
  • Chapter 8 Sampling
  • Chapter 9 Survey Research
  • Chapter 10 Experimental Research
  • Chapter 11 Case Research
  • Chapter 12 Interpretive Research
  • Chapter 13 Qualitative Analysis
  • Chapter 14 Quantitative Analysis Descriptive Statistics
  • Chapter 15 Quantitative Analysis Inferential Statistics
  • Chapter 16 Research Ethics
  • About the Book
  • About the Author

Course Information

  • Welcome to the Course!
  • The textbook for this course is an Open Educational Resource.
  • Prof Pelz Contact Information
  • What is Heutagogy?
  • Course Learning Activities
  • How Your Course Grade is Determined
  • My Expectations of your committment to this class.
  • Course Objectives, Student Learning Outcomes, Assessment

Icebreaker Activities

  • Poke around...
  • Open SUNY Student Orientation
  • Practice participating in a Q & A Discussion Forum
  • Talk with the Professor (IB)

Modules 1 through 5

  • Learning Module Structure
  • Module Overview
  • F1: Chapters 1, 2, 3 & 4 Discussion Forum
  • Instructions for completing the Reflective Blog
  • BD1: Blog 1 Discussion

Research Portfolio - Submit and discuss your Portfolio of Research Articles

  • Instructions for completing the Research Portfolio assignment
  • Research Portfolio Discussion Forums
  • Culminating Activities
  • Rate your Professor...
  • Experimental Psychology Course Survey

This courseware includes resources copyrighted and openly licensed by multiple individuals and organizations. Click the words "Licenses and Attributions" at the bottom of each page for copyright and licensing information specific to the material on that page. If you believe that this courseware violates your copyright, please contact us .

Cover Image: "Landscapes and Monochromes." Authored by: Anna Ostoya. Provided by: The ING Polish Art Foundation. Located at: https://ingart.pl/en/artists/anna-ostoya . Content Type: CC Licensed Content, Specific Attribution. License: CC BY: Attribution . License Terms: Creative Commons Licence 3.0.

Lumen Learning

Lumen Learning provides a simple, supported path for faculty members to adopt and teach effectively with open educational resources (OER). Read more about what we do.

Digital Commons @ University of South Florida

  • USF Research
  • USF Libraries
  • < Previous

Digital Commons @ USF > USF Libraries > Tampa Library > Textbooks > 3

  • Textbooks Collection

Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices

Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices

Anol Bhattacherjee , University of South Florida Follow

Download Full Text (2.0 MB)

Download Translated in Arabic (3.9 MB)

Download Translated in Chinese (3.6 MB)

Download Translated in Korean (4.3 MB)

Download Translated in Persian (2.4 MB)

Download Translated in Turkish (4.4 MB)

Download Translated in Ukrainian (2.0 MB)

Download Translated in Vietnamese (6.6 MB)

Description

This book is designed to introduce doctoral and graduate students to the process of scientific research in the social sciences, business, education, public health, and related disciplines. It is a one-stop, comprehensive, and compact source for foundational concepts in behavioral research, and can serve as a stand-alone text or as a supplement to research readings in any course on research methods.

The contents and examples are designed for anyone interested in behavioral research (not just information systems people), and so, the book should appeal to most business programs, social sciences, education, public health, and related disciplines.

Publication Date

Global Text Project

Disciplines

American Studies | Arts and Humanities | Education | Public Health | Social and Behavioral Sciences

Recommended Citation

Bhattacherjee, Anol, "Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices" (2012). Textbooks Collection . 3. https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/oa_textbooks/3

Since November 22, 2011

Included in

American Studies Commons , Education Commons , Public Health Commons , Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons

Advanced Search

  • Email Notifications and RSS
  • All Collections
  • USF Faculty Publications
  • Open Access Journals
  • Conferences and Events
  • Theses and Dissertations

Useful Links

  • Tampa Library Home
  • Rights Information
  • SelectedWorks
  • Submit Research

Home | About | Help | My Account | Accessibility Statement | Language and Diversity Statements

Privacy Copyright

Ask A Librarian

  • Collections
  • Research Help
  • Teaching & Learning
  • Library Home

Social Sciences Research Methods

  • Using the WVU Libraries

Research Design

  • Quantitative Methods

Qualitative Methods

Sources cited.

  • Find Articles that use Methods
  • Data & Visualization
  • Peer-Reviewed Sources

Introductions to quantitative & qualitative methodologies

The following sources provide explanations, examples and procedures about various methodologies used in the social sciences research.

  • Sage Research Methods This link opens in a new window (SRM) is the major tool for exploring various methodologies. SRM includes the Little Green and Little Blue book series, articles, case studies, databases and videos covering quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods research methodologies.
  • Research Design : Creating Robust Approaches for the Social Sciences (in SRM) "The book combines being a good read for experienced researchers who want to remind themselves of how to design good quality, rigorous, research, with being an excellent introduction for those new to the field in how to plan and execute inherently useful research for any evidence base."
  • Dictionary of Social Research Methods "This dictionary offers succinct, clear, expert explanations of key terms from both method and methodology in social research."
  • When to Use What Research Design "Systematic, practical, and accessible, this is the first book to focus on finding the most defensible design for a particular research question."

Quantitative Methods  

  • Analyzing Quantitative Data : An Introduction for Social Researchers "The book guides readers through the steps of data analysis, from organizing raw data to utilizing descriptive statistics and tests of significance, drawing valid conclusions, and writing research reports."
  • Basic statistics for social research Introduction to "core general statistical concepts and methods" in the social sciences.

Cover Art

  • Doing Surveys Online (in SRM) How-to guide covers "the entire process of using surveys, from systematically recruiting respondents, to designing the internet survey, to processing the survey data and writing it up."
  • Fundamentals of Qualitative Research A "concise yet rigorous description of how to design and conduct fieldwork projects and how to examine data in multiple ways for interpretive insight."
  • Qualitative Research for the Social Sciences (in SRM) Covers "the full range of qualitative methodologies" along with "step-by-step coverage of the research process, addressing issues of analyzing data, presenting completed research, and evaluating research."
  • Qualitative Research Practice "The essential reference for anyone undertaking or studying qualitative research. It covers a diversity of methods and a variety of perspectives and is a very practical and informative guide for newcomers and experienced researchers alike."
  • SAGE Handbook of Qualitative Data Collection (in SRM) "Systematically explores the approaches, techniques, debates and new frontiers for creating, collecting and producing qualitative data."

Thank you to Emily Keller, Political Science and Public Policy Librarian at the University of Washington Libraries who allowed me to copy parts of of her Social Science Research Methods guide for this page. 

  • << Previous: Using the WVU Libraries
  • Next: Find Articles that use Methods >>
  • Last Updated: Feb 6, 2024 11:20 AM
  • URL: https://libguides.wvu.edu/SocialScienceMethods
  • Find My Rep

You are here

The SAGE Handbook of Social Research Methods

The SAGE Handbook of Social Research Methods

  • Pertti Alasuutari - University of Tampere, Finland
  • Leonard Bickman - Vanderbilt University, USA and Florida International University, USA
  • Julia Brannen - Insitute of Education, University of London
  • Description

The Handbook includes chapters on each phase of the research process: research design, methods of data collection, and the processes of analyzing and interpreting data. The volume maintains that there is much more to research than learning skills and techniques; methodology involves the fit between theory, research questions research design and analysis. The book also includes several chapters that describe historical and current directions in social research, debating crucial subjects such as qualitative versus quantitative paradigms, how to judge the credibility of types of research, and the increasingly topical issue of research ethics.

The Handbook serves as an invaluable resource for approaching research with an open mind. This volume maps the field of social research methods using an approach that will prove valuable for both students and researchers.   Social Research in Changing Social Conditions   PART ONE: DIRECTIONS IN SOCIAL RESEARCH Alan Bryman The End of the Paradigm Wars? Marja Alastalo The History of Social Research Methods Martyn Hammersley Assessing Validity in Social Research Karen Armstrong Ethnography and Audience Pekka Sulkunen Social Research and Social Practice in Post-Positivist Society Ann Nilsen From Questions of Methods to Epistemological Issues The Case of Biographical Research

I would highly recommend this book as it is easy to understand and covers all the main issues of social research.

Great resource book for introductory research methods. Students really like the chapters.

This text is a valuable reference for social science research. The section on analysis and interpretation of evidence is particularly valuable as it affords opportunities for students to consider how they might want to organise their research design differently in order to draw upon a wider range of analysis methods, that are often less represented in other texts.

This is an excellent text used to navigate students through the research process required for their ethical proposal and dissertation.

Excellent book that is a vital component of any social research methods class. Covers a wide array of research topics in an accessible format.

Comprehensive text. Particularly interesting was the chapter on paradigms. Not as practical as expected, however, content is good.

This is an easy to read to text and I have adopted this as one of the key essential texts for this unit.

Interesting mid-level discussion of research methods; includes examples from my field (education) but they are situated within the larger context of social science research. Thanks for this one!

Give a great all round review of all aspects of social science, and despite its focus on sociology it appears generally applicable to other aspects of social sciences.

Under the current course design, this book, whilst an excellent text, is a step too far too soon for the students and can act only as a terciary reading text... which, if I am being honest, means it will be barely looked at. Consequently, whilst included in the reading list I cannot see it being used by anyone but me. The module is undr review though (for expansion) and it may take on greater promenance in the wider design.

Preview this book

For instructors, select a purchasing option, order from:.

  • VitalSource
  • Amazon Kindle
  • Google Play

Related Products

Social Network Analysis

SAGE Knowledge is the premier social sciences platform for SAGE and CQ Press book, reference and video content.

The platform allows researchers to cross-search and seamlessly access a wide breadth of must-have SAGE book and reference content from one source.

SAGE Research Methods Promotion

SAGE Research Methods is a research methods tool created to help researchers, faculty and students with their research projects. SAGE Research Methods links over 175,000 pages of SAGE’s renowned book, journal and reference content with truly advanced search and discovery tools. Researchers can explore methods concepts to help them design research projects, understand particular methods or identify a new method, conduct their research, and write up their findings. Since SAGE Research Methods focuses on methodology rather than disciplines, it can be used across the social sciences, health sciences, and more.

With SAGE Research Methods, researchers can explore their chosen method across the depth and breadth of content, expanding or refining their search as needed; read online, print, or email full-text content; utilize suggested related methods and links to related authors from SAGE Research Methods' robust library and unique features; and even share their own collections of content through Methods Lists. SAGE Research Methods contains content from over 720 books, dictionaries, encyclopedias, and handbooks, the entire “Little Green Book,” and "Little Blue Book” series, two Major Works collating a selection of journal articles, and specially commissioned videos.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

Research Methods in the Social Sciences: An A-Z of key concepts

research methods for social sciences

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Perspect Clin Res
  • v.14(1); Jan-Mar 2023
  • PMC10003579

Introduction to qualitative research methods – Part I

Shagufta bhangu.

Department of Global Health and Social Medicine, King's College London, London, United Kingdom

Fabien Provost

Carlo caduff.

Qualitative research methods are widely used in the social sciences and the humanities, but they can also complement quantitative approaches used in clinical research. In this article, we discuss the key features and contributions of qualitative research methods.

INTRODUCTION

Qualitative research methods refer to techniques of investigation that rely on nonstatistical and nonnumerical methods of data collection, analysis, and evidence production. Qualitative research techniques provide a lens for learning about nonquantifiable phenomena such as people's experiences, languages, histories, and cultures. In this article, we describe the strengths and role of qualitative research methods and how these can be employed in clinical research.

Although frequently employed in the social sciences and humanities, qualitative research methods can complement clinical research. These techniques can contribute to a better understanding of the social, cultural, political, and economic dimensions of health and illness. Social scientists and scholars in the humanities rely on a wide range of methods, including interviews, surveys, participant observation, focus groups, oral history, and archival research to examine both structural conditions and lived experience [ Figure 1 ]. Such research can not only provide robust and reliable data but can also humanize and add richness to our understanding of the ways in which people in different parts of the world perceive and experience illness and how they interact with medical institutions, systems, and therapeutics.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is PCR-14-39-g001.jpg

Examples of qualitative research techniques

Qualitative research methods should not be seen as tools that can be applied independently of theory. It is important for these tools to be based on more than just method. In their research, social scientists and scholars in the humanities emphasize social theory. Departing from a reductionist psychological model of individual behavior that often blames people for their illness, social theory focuses on relations – disease happens not simply in people but between people. This type of theoretically informed and empirically grounded research thus examines not just patients but interactions between a wide range of actors (e.g., patients, family members, friends, neighbors, local politicians, medical practitioners at all levels, and from many systems of medicine, researchers, policymakers) to give voice to the lived experiences, motivations, and constraints of all those who are touched by disease.

PHILOSOPHICAL FOUNDATIONS OF QUALITATIVE RESEARCH METHODS

In identifying the factors that contribute to the occurrence and persistence of a phenomenon, it is paramount that we begin by asking the question: what do we know about this reality? How have we come to know this reality? These two processes, which we can refer to as the “what” question and the “how” question, are the two that all scientists (natural and social) grapple with in their research. We refer to these as the ontological and epistemological questions a research study must address. Together, they help us create a suitable methodology for any research study[ 1 ] [ Figure 2 ]. Therefore, as with quantitative methods, there must be a justifiable and logical method for understanding the world even for qualitative methods. By engaging with these two dimensions, the ontological and the epistemological, we open a path for learning that moves away from commonsensical understandings of the world, and the perpetuation of stereotypes and toward robust scientific knowledge production.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is PCR-14-39-g002.jpg

Developing a research methodology

Every discipline has a distinct research philosophy and way of viewing the world and conducting research. Philosophers and historians of science have extensively studied how these divisions and specializations have emerged over centuries.[ 1 , 2 , 3 ] The most important distinction between quantitative and qualitative research techniques lies in the nature of the data they study and analyze. While the former focus on statistical, numerical, and quantitative aspects of phenomena and employ the same in data collection and analysis, qualitative techniques focus on humanistic, descriptive, and qualitative aspects of phenomena.[ 4 ]

For the findings of any research study to be reliable, they must employ the appropriate research techniques that are uniquely tailored to the phenomena under investigation. To do so, researchers must choose techniques based on their specific research questions and understand the strengths and limitations of the different tools available to them. Since clinical work lies at the intersection of both natural and social phenomena, it means that it must study both: biological and physiological phenomena (natural, quantitative, and objective phenomena) and behavioral and cultural phenomena (social, qualitative, and subjective phenomena). Therefore, clinical researchers can gain from both sets of techniques in their efforts to produce medical knowledge and bring forth scientifically informed change.

KEY FEATURES AND CONTRIBUTIONS OF QUALITATIVE RESEARCH METHODS

In this section, we discuss the key features and contributions of qualitative research methods [ Figure 3 ]. We describe the specific strengths and limitations of these techniques and discuss how they can be deployed in scientific investigations.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is PCR-14-39-g003.jpg

Key features of qualitative research methods

One of the most important contributions of qualitative research methods is that they provide rigorous, theoretically sound, and rational techniques for the analysis of subjective, nebulous, and difficult-to-pin-down phenomena. We are aware, for example, of the role that social factors play in health care but find it hard to qualify and quantify these in our research studies. Often, we find researchers basing their arguments on “common sense,” developing research studies based on assumptions about the people that are studied. Such commonsensical assumptions are perhaps among the greatest impediments to knowledge production. For example, in trying to understand stigma, surveys often make assumptions about its reasons and frequently associate it with vague and general common sense notions of “fear” and “lack of information.” While these may be at work, to make such assumptions based on commonsensical understandings, and without conducting research inhibit us from exploring the multiple social factors that are at work under the guise of stigma.

In unpacking commonsensical understandings and researching experiences, relationships, and other phenomena, qualitative researchers are assisted by their methodological commitment to open-ended research. By open-ended research, we mean that these techniques take on an unbiased and exploratory approach in which learnings from the field and from research participants, are recorded and analyzed to learn about the world.[ 5 ] This orientation is made possible by qualitative research techniques that are particularly effective in learning about specific social, cultural, economic, and political milieus.

Second, qualitative research methods equip us in studying complex phenomena. Qualitative research methods provide scientific tools for exploring and identifying the numerous contributing factors to an occurrence. Rather than establishing one or the other factor as more important, qualitative methods are open-ended, inductive (ground-up), and empirical. They allow us to understand the object of our analysis from multiple vantage points and in its dispersion and caution against predetermined notions of the object of inquiry. They encourage researchers instead to discover a reality that is not yet given, fixed, and predetermined by the methods that are used and the hypotheses that underlie the study.

Once the multiple factors at work in a phenomenon have been identified, we can employ quantitative techniques and embark on processes of measurement, establish patterns and regularities, and analyze the causal and correlated factors at work through statistical techniques. For example, a doctor may observe that there is a high patient drop-out in treatment. Before carrying out a study which relies on quantitative techniques, qualitative research methods such as conversation analysis, interviews, surveys, or even focus group discussions may prove more effective in learning about all the factors that are contributing to patient default. After identifying the multiple, intersecting factors, quantitative techniques can be deployed to measure each of these factors through techniques such as correlational or regression analyses. Here, the use of quantitative techniques without identifying the diverse factors influencing patient decisions would be premature. Qualitative techniques thus have a key role to play in investigations of complex realities and in conducting rich exploratory studies while embracing rigorous and philosophically grounded methodologies.

Third, apart from subjective, nebulous, and complex phenomena, qualitative research techniques are also effective in making sense of irrational, illogical, and emotional phenomena. These play an important role in understanding logics at work among patients, their families, and societies. Qualitative research techniques are aided by their ability to shift focus away from the individual as a unit of analysis to the larger social, cultural, political, economic, and structural forces at work in health. As health-care practitioners and researchers focused on biological, physiological, disease and therapeutic processes, sociocultural, political, and economic conditions are often peripheral or ignored in day-to-day clinical work. However, it is within these latter processes that both health-care practices and patient lives are entrenched. Qualitative researchers are particularly adept at identifying the structural conditions such as the social, cultural, political, local, and economic conditions which contribute to health care and experiences of disease and illness.

For example, the decision to delay treatment by a patient may be understood as an irrational choice impacting his/her chances of survival, but the same may be a result of the patient treating their child's education as a financial priority over his/her own health. While this appears as an “emotional” choice, qualitative researchers try to understand the social and cultural factors that structure, inform, and justify such choices. Rather than assuming that it is an irrational choice, qualitative researchers try to understand the norms and logical grounds on which the patient is making this decision. By foregrounding such logics, stories, fears, and desires, qualitative research expands our analytic precision in learning about complex social worlds, recognizing reasons for medical successes and failures, and interrogating our assumptions about human behavior. These in turn can prove useful in arriving at conclusive, actionable findings which can inform institutional and public health policies and have a very important role to play in any change and transformation we may wish to bring to the societies in which we work.

Financial support and sponsorship

Conflicts of interest.

There are no conflicts of interest.

  • I'M AN INSTRUCTOR
  • I'M A STUDENT

United States Store

Find what you need to succeed.

  • Our Mission
  • Our Leadership
  • Learning Science
  • Macmillan Learning AI
  • Sustainability
  • Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
  • Accessibility
  • Astronomy Biochemistry Biology Chemistry College Success Communication Economics Electrical Engineering English Environmental Science Geography Geology History Mathematics Music & Theater Nutrition and Health Philosophy & Religion Physics Psychology Sociology Statistics Value
  • Digital Offerings
  • Inclusive Access
  • Lab Solutions
  • LMS Integration
  • Curriculum Solutions
  • Training and Demos
  • First Day of Class
  • Administrators
  • Affordable Solutions
  • Badging & Certification
  • News & Media
  • Contact Us & FAQs
  • Find Your Rep
  • Booksellers
  • Macmillan International Support
  • International Translation Rights
  • Request Permissions
  • Report Piracy

Research Methods in the Social Sciences

Cover: Research Methods in the Social Sciences, 8th Edition by Chava Frankfort-Nachmias; David Nachmias; Jack DeWaard

Psychology in Everyday Life

Eighth edition | ©2015 chava frankfort-nachmias; david nachmias; jack dewaard.

ISBN:9781429233002

Read and study old-school with our bound texts.

With its clear presentation and integration of detailed real-world examples, the acclaimed Research Methods in the Social Sciences accessibly illustrates the relevance of social sciences research without sacrificing key content. The eighth edition includes engaging new studies and examples that explore the use of Twitter feeds in social sciences research, the effect of climate change on human conflict, the use of rational intuition in explaining the Chelyabinsk meteor explosion, and much more. The eighth edition also includes new and expanded coverage of conducting qualitative research, as well as conducting research in our digital age, including the use of the Internet and various computer software packages for retrieving, cleaning, coding, and analyzing "big data."

New to This Edition

Eighth Edition | ©2015

Chava Frankfort-Nachmias; David Nachmias; Jack DeWaard

Digital Options

Eighth Edition | 2015

Table of Contents

Headshot of Chava Frankfort-Nachmias

Chava Frankfort-Nachmias

Chava Frankfort-Nachmias is an Emeritus Professor of Sociology at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee. In addition to Research Methods in the Social Sciences, she is coauthor of Social Statistics for a Diverse Society, coeditor of Sappho in the Holy Land (with Erella Shadmi) and numerous publications on ethnicity and development, urban revitalization, science and gender, and women in Israel. She was the recipient of the University of Wisconsin System teaching improvement grant on integrating race, ethnicity, and gender into the social statistics and research methods curriculum.

Headshot of David Nachmias

David Nachmias

David Nachmias is a Senior Fellow Emeritus at the Israel Democracy Institute (IDI) and holds the Romulo Betancourt Chair in Political Science at Tel Aviv University. Professor Nachmias has extensively published and presented papers in the areas of Political Science, Public Administration and Public Policy, both in Israel and abroad. He now serves on the editorial board of Policy Studies Review; and is a member of the American Political Science Association; Midwest Political Science Association; Policy Studies Organization; the International Political Science Association and Israels Political Science Association. His numerous books and articles include: Public Policy in Israel, Frank Cass, 2002; Executive Governance in Israel, Patgrave, 2002 "The Bias of Pluralism: The Redistributive Consequences of Israels New Electoral Law" in A. Arian and Michal Shamir (eds.) The Elections in Israel - 1996, State University of New York Press, 1999.

Headshot of Jack DeWaard

Jack DeWaard

Jack DeWaard is an Associate Professor in the Department of Sociology and Graduate Faculty in Population Studies in the Minnesota Population Center at University of Minnesota who specializes in international and internal migration, racial and ethnic stratification and inequality, demography and ecology, and quantitative methods. DeWaard teaches graduate and undergraduate courses on research methods.

Eighth Edition | 2015

Instructor Resources

Need instructor resources for your course, download resources.

You need to sign in to unlock your resources.

Illustration Slides

Instructor's resource manual in pdf, instructor's resource manual in word, lecture slides.

You've selected:

Click the E-mail Download Link button and we'll send you an e-mail at with links to download your instructor resources. Please note there may be a delay in delivering your e-mail depending on the size of the files.

Your download request has been received and your download link will be sent to .

Please note you could wait up to 30 to 60 minutes to receive your download e-mail depending on the number and size of the files. We appreciate your patience while we process your request.

Check your inbox, trash, and spam folders for an e-mail from [email protected] .

If you do not receive your e-mail, please visit macmillanlearning.com/support .

Related Titles

Select a demo to view:

We are happy to offer free Achieve access in addition to the physical sample you have selected. Sample this version now as opposed to waiting for the physical edition.

Social Science Methodology

Maxwell school news and commentary.

John Gerring, Boston University

Cambridge Press University, February 2012

Center for Qualitative and Multi-Method Inquiry

Research Methods

Cambridge Press University

Cover of the book Social Science Methodology

John Gerring's exceptional textbook has been thoroughly revised in this second edition. It offers a one-volume introduction to social science methodology relevant to the disciplines of anthropology, economics, history, political science, psychology and sociology. This new edition has been extensively developed with the introduction of new material and a thorough treatment of essential elements such as conceptualization, measurement, causality and research design. It is written for students, long-time practitioners and methodologists and covers both qualitative and quantitative methods. It synthesizes the vast and diverse field of methodology in a way that is clear, concise and comprehensive. While offering a handy overview of the subject, the book is also an argument about how we should conceptualize methodological problems. Thinking about methodology through this lens provides a new framework for understanding work in the social sciences.

Related News

Nicholas Oesterling headshot

Jun 21, 2024

Understanding the Emergence of Computational Institutional Science

Scott Landes

Dec 6, 2023

Counting Disability in the National Health Interview Survey and Its Consequence

Disability , Longevity , Research Methods , United States

Oct 2, 2023

Health Equity for People With IDD Requires Vast Improvements to Data Collection

COVID-19 , IDD , Research Methods , United States

gadarian-shana-oxford-handbook-political-psychology

Sep 21, 2023

Emotion and Political Psychology

Media & Journalism , Mental Health , Political Parties , Research Methods

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 31 July 2024

Liars know they are lying: differentiating disinformation from disagreement

  • Stephan Lewandowsky   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-1655-2013 1 , 2 ,
  • Ullrich K. H. Ecker   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4743-313X 3 ,
  • John Cook 4 ,
  • Sander van der Linden 5 ,
  • Jon Roozenbeek 6 ,
  • Naomi Oreskes 7 &
  • Lee C. McIntyre 8  

Humanities and Social Sciences Communications volume  11 , Article number:  986 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

61 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Politics and international relations

Mis- and disinformation pose substantial societal challenges, and have thus become the focus of a substantive field of research. However, the field of misinformation research has recently come under scrutiny on two fronts. First, a political response has emerged, claiming that misinformation research aims to censor conservative voices. Second, some scholars have questioned the utility of misinformation research altogether, arguing that misinformation is not sufficiently identifiable or widespread to warrant much concern or action. Here, we rebut these claims. We contend that the spread of misinformation—and in particular willful disinformation—is demonstrably harmful to public health, evidence-informed policymaking, and democratic processes. We also show that disinformation and outright lies can often be identified and differ from good-faith political contestation. We conclude by showing how misinformation and disinformation can be at least partially mitigated using a variety of empirically validated, rights-preserving methods that do not involve censorship.

Similar content being viewed by others

research methods for social sciences

A meta-analysis of correction effects in science-relevant misinformation

research methods for social sciences

Understanding belief in political statements using a model-driven experimental approach: a registered report

research methods for social sciences

Framing fact-checks as a “confirmation” increases engagement with corrections of misinformation: a four-country study

“If everybody always lies to you, the consequence is not that you believe the lies, but rather that nobody believes anything any longer.”
— Hannah Arendt

One of the normative goods on which democracy relies is accountable representation through fair elections (Tenove, 2020 ). This good is at risk when public perception of the integrity of elections is significantly distorted by false or misleading information (H. Farrell and Schneier, 2018 ). The two most recent presidential elections in the U.S. were accompanied by a plethora of false or misleading information, which grew from false information about voting procedures in 2016 (Stapleton, 2016 ) to the “big lie” that the 2020 election was stolen from Donald Trump, which he and his allies have baselessly and ceaselessly repeated (Henricksen and Betz, 2023 ; Jacobson, 2023 ). Misleading or false information has always been part and parcel of political debate (Lewandowsky et al., 2017 ), and the public arguably accepts a certain amount of dishonesty from politicians (e.g., McGraw, 1998 ; Swire-Thompson et al., 2020 ). However, Trump’s big lie differs from conventional, often accidentally disseminated, mis information by being a deliberate attempt to dis inform the public.

Scholars tend to think of disinformation as a type of misinformation and technically that is true: intentional falsehoods are but one subset of falsehoods (Lewandowsky et al., 2013 ) and intentionality does not affect how people’s cognitive apparatus processes the information (e.g., L. K. Fazio et al., 2015 ). But given the real-world risks that disinformation poses for democracy (Lewandowsky et al., 2023 ), we think it is important to be clear at the outset whether we are dealing with a mistake versus a lie.

The tobacco industry’s 50-year-long campaign of disinformation about the health risks from smoking is a classic case of deliberate deception and has been recognized as such by the U.S. Federal Courts (Smith et al., 2011 , see also Civil Action 99-2496(GK) United States District Court, District of Columbia. United States v. Philip Morris Inc.). This article focuses primarily on the nature of disinformation and how it can be identified, and places it into the contemporary societal context. Wherever we make a broader point about the prevalence of false information, its identifiability or its effects, we use the term misinformation to indicate that intentionality is secondary or unknown.

An analysis of mis- and disinformation cannot be complete without also considering the role of the audience, in particular when people share information with others, where the distinction between mis- and disinformation becomes more fluid. In most instances, when people share information, they do so based on the justifiable default expectation that it is true (Grice, 1975 ). However, occasionally people also share information that they know to be false, a phenomenon known as “participatory propaganda” (e.g., Lewandowsky, 2022 ; Wanless and Berk, 2019 ). One factor that may underlie participatory propaganda is the social utility that persons can derive from beliefs, even if they are false, which may stimulate them into rationalizing belief in falsehoods (Williams, 2022 ). The converse may also occur, where members of the public accurately report an experience, which is then taken up by others, usually political operatives or elites, and redeployed for a malign purpose. For example, technical problems with some voting machines in Arizona in 2022 were seized on by Trump and his allies as being an attempt to disenfranchise conservative voters (Reid, 2022 ). Both cases underscore the importance of audience involvement and the reverberating feedback loops between political actors and the public which can often amplify and extend the reach of intentional disinformation (Starbird et al., 2023 ; Vosoughi et al., 2018 ), and which can often involve non-epistemic but nonetheless rational choices (Williams, 2021 , 2022 ).

The circular and mutually reinforcing relationship between political actors and the public was a particularly pernicious aspect of the rhetoric associated with Trump’s big lie (for a detailed analysis, see Starbird et al., 2023 ). During the joint session of Congress to certify the election on 6 January 2021, politicians speaking in support of Donald Trump and his unsubstantiated claims about election irregularities appealed not to evidence or facts but to public opinion. For example, Senator Ted Cruz cited a poll result that 39% of the public believed the election had been “rigged”. Similarly, Representative Jim Jordan (R-Ohio), who is now Chairman of the House Judiciary Committee, argued against certification of the election by arguing that “80 million of our fellow citizens, Republicans and Democrats, have doubts about this election; and 60 million people, 60 million Americans think it was stolen” (Salek, 2023 ). The appeal to public opinion to buttress false claims is cynical in light of the fact that public opinion was the result of systematic disinformation in the first place. While nearly 75% of Republicans considered the election result legitimate on election day, this share dropped to around 40% within a few days (Arceneaux and Truex, 2022 ), coinciding with the period during which Trump ramped up his false claims about the election being stolen. By December 2020, 28% of American conservatives did not support a peaceful transfer of power (Weinschenk et al., 2021 ), perhaps the most important bedrock of democracy. Among liberals, by contrast, this attitude was far more marginal (3%).

Public opinion has shifted remarkably little since the election. In August 2023, nearly 70% of Republican voters continued to question the legitimacy of President Biden’s electoral win in 2020. More than half of those who questioned Biden’s win believed that there was solid evidence proving that the election was not legitimate (Agiesta and Edwards-Levy, 2023 ). However, the purported evidence marshaled in support of this view has been repeatedly shown to be false (Canon and Sherman, 2021 ; Eggers et al., 2021 ; Grofman and Cervas, 2023 ). Footnote 1 It is particularly striking that high levels of false election beliefs are found even under conditions known to reduce “expressive responding”—that is, responses that express support for a position but do not reflect true belief (Graham and Yair, 2023 ).

The entrenchment of the big lie erodes the core of American democracy and puts pressure on Republican politicians to cater to antidemocratic forces (Arceneaux and Truex, 2022 ; Jacobson, 2021 , 2023 ). It has demonstrably decreased trust in the electoral system (Berlinski et al., 2021 ), and a violent constitutional crisis has been identified as a “tail risk” for the United States in 2024 (McLauchlin, 2023 ). Similar crises in which right-wing authoritarian movements are dismantling democratic institutions and safeguards have found traction in many countries around the world including liberal democracies (Cooley and Nexon, 2022 ).

In this context, it is worth noting that the situation in other countries, notably in the Global South, may differ from the situation in the U.S. (Badrinathan and Chauchard, 2024 ). On the one hand, low state capacity and infrastructure constraints may curtail the ability of powerful actors to spread disinformation and propaganda (though see Kellow and Steeves, 1998 ; Li, 2004 , for discussion of the role of government-adjacent radio station RTLM in facilitating the 1994 Rwandan genocide). On the other hand, such spread can be facilitated by the fact that closed, encrypted social-media channels are particularly popular in the Global South, sometimes providing an alternative source of news when broadcast channels and other conventional media have limited reach. In those cases, dissemination strategies will also be less direct, relying more on distributed “cyber-armies” than direct one-to-millions broadcasts such as Trump’s social-media posts (Badrinathan, 2021 ; Jalli and Idris, 2019 ). The harm that can be caused by such distributed systems was vividly illustrated by the false rumors about child kidnapers shared in Indian WhatsApp groups in 2018, which incited at least 16 mob lynchings, causing the deaths of 29 innocent people (Dixit and Mac, 2018 ). The ensuing interplay between the attempts of the Indian government to hold WhatsApp accountable and Meta, the platform’s owner, highlights the limited power that governments in the Global South hold over multinational technology corporations (Arun, 2019 ). As a result, many platforms do not even have moderation tools for problematic content in popular non-Western languages (Shahid and Vashistha, 2023 ).

The power asymmetry between corporations and the Global South has been noted repeatedly, and recent calls for action include the idea of collective action by countries in the Global South to insist on regulation of platforms (Takhshid, 2021 ). We have only scratched the surface of a big global issue that is in urgent need of being addressed.

Despite these differences between the Global North and South, beliefs in political misinformation can be pervasive regardless of regime type or development level (e.g., for a discussion in the context of the “developing democracy” of Brazil, see Dourado and Salgado, 2021 ; Pereira et al., 2022 ).

The political landscape of disinformation

Given that the 2020 election was lost by the Republican candidate, the finding that conservatives are more likely than liberals to believe false election claims is explainable on the basis of motivated cognition and the general finding that conspiracy theories “are for losers” (Uscinski and Parent, 2014 ); that is, they provide an explanation—even if only a chimerical one—for a political setback to the losing parties. There is no a priori reason to assume that susceptibility to disinformation is skewed across the political spectrum.

However, a large body of recent research on the American public and U.S. political actors has consistently identified a pervasive ideological asymmetry, with conservatives and people from the populist right being far more likely to consume, share, and believe false information than their liberal counterparts (Benkler et al., 2018 ; Garrett and Bond, 2021 ; González-Bailón et al., 2023 ; Grinberg et al., 2019 ; Guess et al., 2020a ; Guess et al., 2020b ; Guess et al., 2019 ; Ognyanova et al., 2020 ). Research into the asymmetry culminated in a recent analysis of the news diet of 208 million Facebook users in the U.S., which discovered that a substantial segment of the news ecosystem is consumed exclusively by conservatives and that most misinformation exists within this ideological bubble (González-Bailón et al., 2023 ). Although the reasons for this asymmetry are not fully understood, Lasser et al. ( 2022 ) recently showed that it also held for politicians, with Republican members of Congress disseminating far more low-quality information on Twitter/X than their Democratic counterparts. Greene ( 2024 ) reported a parallel analysis for Facebook and found the same asymmetry between politicians of the two major parties. Similarly, Benkler et al. ( 2018 ) showed how the particular structure of the American media scene, with a dense interconnected cluster of right-wing sources that is separate from the remaining mainstream, fosters political asymmetry in the use and consumption of disinformation.

This asymmetry extends beyond the political domain to health-related information, which might at first glance appear to be of sufficient importance for most people to cast aside their political leanings. A recent systematic review discovered eight studies that identified conservatism as a predictor of susceptibility to health misinformation, seven studies that found no association involving political leanings, and not a single study that showed liberals to be more misinformed on health topics than conservatives (Nan et al., 2022 ). The observed political asymmetry is also not limited to survey results or other behavioral measures. Wallace et al. ( 2023 ) examined vaccination and mortality data from two U.S. states (Ohio and Florida) during the COVID-19 pandemic and found a widening partisan gap in excess mortality. Specifically, whereas mortality rates were equal for registered Republican and Democratic voters pre-pandemic, a wide partisan gap—with excess death rates among Republicans being up to 43% greater than among Democratic voters—was observed after vaccines had become available for everyone. The gap was greatest in counties with the lowest share of vaccinated people and it almost disappeared for the most vaccinated counties. Similar results have been reported across U.S. states (Leonhardt, 2021 ). One explanation for these patterns invokes the frequent false statements by Republican politicians and conservative news networks—foremost Fox News—that discredited the COVID-19 vaccines (Hotez, 2023 ). In support, consumption of Fox News has been causally linked to lower vaccination rates (Pinna et al., 2022 ).

Moreover, a recent analysis identified a specific “Trump effect” such that even conditional on the Republican vote share, support for Trump was additionally and causally associated with a lower vaccination rate (Jung and Lee, 2023 ).

The political asymmetry surrounding the dissemination and consumption of misinformation must be caveated in two ways. First, although the asymmetry is substantial and pervasive it is not absolute. For some materials, such as specific conspiracy theories, the asymmetry is found to be attenuated in some studies (A. Enders et al., 2022 ; M. Enders and Uscinski, 2021 ). Second, the asymmetry observed among American politicians does not necessarily hold in other countries. Lasser et al. ( 2022 ) examined tweets by British and German parliamentarians and showed that with the exception of the extreme right in Germany (the AfD party), politicians across the mainstream spectrum were equally judicious in what information they shared in their tweets. This finding suggests that it is not conservatism per se that is associated with asymmetric reliance on misinformation, but the specific manifestation of conservatism currently dominant in the American political landscape.

Notwithstanding those caveats, the political asymmetry surrounding the dissemination and consumption of misinformation in the U.S. has been accompanied by at least two major issues: First, there has been a strong political response by Republicans in Congress who have commenced a campaign against misinformation research and researchers, claiming that the research seeks to censor conservative voices. Second, the political backlash has coincided with growing self-reflection and critique among scholars, some of whom began to question the misinformation research effort, culminating in claims that misinformation may not be sufficiently identifiable or widespread to warrant concern or countermeasures. We now take up these two issues in turn.

The politicization of misinformation research

At the time of this writing, Representative Jim Jordan, R-Ohio, has been leading a campaign against misinformation research and misinformation researchers in his role as Chairman of the House Judiciary Committee. The core allegation by Jordan and his allies Footnote 2 is that misinformation researchers are part of a purported “Censorship Industrial Complex” that is assisting the Biden administration in its purported endeavor to pressure platforms into suppressing conservative viewpoints (U.S. House of Representatives Judiciary Committee, 2023 ). The allegation is, however, problematic for at least four reasons: it rests on false assertions; it ironically denies first-amendment rights to researchers; it rests on a basic premise that is false; and it misunderstands the role of platforms in content moderation.

Concerning the first point, Jordan has subpoenaed several prominent academics engaged in the study of mis- and disinformation based on false assertions. For example, Dr Kate Starbird, an expert on disinformation from the University of Washington, was called to testify before Jordan’s subcommittee and had to defend herself against accusations that she was colluding with the Biden administration in an effort to chill conservative speech (Nix and Menn, 2023 ). Core to the specific allegations against Starbird and her colleagues is a claim—initially voiced by online conspiracy theorists—that they colluded with the Department of Homeland Security to censor 22 million tweets during the 2020 election campaign. In actual fact, the researchers collected 22 million tweets for analysis, and flagged about 3000 (0.0001 of the total) for potential violations of Twitter’s terms of use (Blitzer, 2023 ).

Second, Jordan’s purported championing of free speech is difficult to reconcile with the chilling effect the House Committee’s actions have had on the first-amendment rights of researchers. According to Starbird, “The people that benefit from the spread of disinformation have effectively silenced many of the people that would try to call them out” (Rutenberg and Myers, 2024 ). The deterring effect on the research community is widespread (Bernstein, 2023 ; Nix et al., 2023 ). Similarly, Facebook and YouTube have reversed their restrictions on content claiming that the 2020 election was stolen. Election disinformation, unsurprisingly, has seen an uptick in response (Rutenberg and Myers, 2024 ).

Third, Jordan’s campaign rests on a false premise, namely that social-media platforms are biased against conservatives. Together with other conservative figures such as Tucker Carlson (formerly with Fox News) and Ben Shapiro, Jordan claimed in 2020 that “Big Tech is out to get conservatives”. This claim has been shown to be wrong by several studies. For example, an analysis of Facebook engagements during the 2016 election campaign revealed that conservative outlets (Fox News, Breitbart, and Daily Caller) amassed 839 million interactions, dwarfing more centrist outlets (CNN with 191 million and ABC news with 138 million), and totaling more than the remaining seven mainstream pages in the top 10 (Barrett and Sims, 2021 ). Another analysis involving millions of Twitter users and 6.2 million news articles shared on the platform also found that conservatives enjoy greater algorithmic amplification than people on the political left (Huszár et al., 2022 ). Moreover, the Congressional January 6th Committee detailed the way in which major platforms, including Twitter and Facebook, facilitated the organization of the violent insurrection in a 122-page memo, although much of that information did not make it into the final committee report (Zakrzewski et al., 2023 ). Congressional investigators discovered that the platforms failed to heed their own experts’ warnings about violent rhetoric on their platforms, and selectively failed to enforce existing rules to avoid antagonizing conservatives for fear of reprisals (Zakrzewski et al., 2023 ).

Finally, and perhaps most important, Jordan’s pursuit fails to differentiate between the roles of government and the platforms, and in particular ignores the crucial role that platforms already play in shaping people’s information diet (Lewandowsky et al., 2023a ). In a nutshell, the internet is currently neither unregulated nor is all information on the internet equally free. Instead, nearly all content on social media is curated by algorithms that are designed to maximize dwell time in pursuit of the platforms’ advertising profit (Lewandowsky and Pomerantsev, 2022 ; Wu, 2017 ). Algorithms therefore favor captivating information that keeps users engaged. Unfortunately, human attention is known to be biased towards negative information (Soroka et al., 2019 ), which creates an incentive for platforms to drench users in outrage-evoking content. Similar to junk food that supermarkets strategically place at checkout lanes, the information that is preferentially curated by platforms may satisfy our presumed momentary preferences while reducing our long-term well-being. If platforms were to address their role in those dynamics, for example by redesigning their algorithms, this would hardly constitute censorship. Solving a problem one has caused is good iterative design rather than bias or suppression of opinions. No one would accuse a supermarket of suppressing consumers’ preferences if the checkout lanes put on offer celery instead of chocolate bars.

In summary, far from being a restorative effort in defense of free speech, Jordan’s attacks are reminiscent of similar campaigns launched against inconvenient scientists by the tobacco and fossil-fuel industries (Lewandowsky et al., 2023b ). In all cases scientists have been subject to personal abuse, their email correspondence is hacked or subpoenaed, and allegations are woven together from snippets of decontextualized actions or events (Blitzer, 2023 ). Because these attacks are systemic, the response also requires a systemic approach (Desikan et al., 2023 ). However, any such response seems unlikely to be achievable in the current political landscape. Scientists who work under such challenging conditions must therefore rely on other avenues to protect their integrity. The U.S. National Academy of Sciences has published a list of resources for scientists under attack. Footnote 3 Specific recommendations include responding publicly to valid criticism (without, however, engaging in a long drawn-out direct conversation with an attacker), reporting abusive messages to the authorities, and seeking support from colleagues who have been in similar situations (Kinser, 2020 ).

The attacks have also coincided with moves by the platforms and the courts that align with Jordan’s claims. For example, the major platforms (Meta, Google, Twitter/X, and Amazon) have cut back on the number of staff dedicated to combating hate speech and misinformation (Field and Vanian, 2023 ). Meta (the parent company of Facebook) has been laying off employees in its “content review” team, which had been involved in countering misinformation and disinformation in the 2022 midterm election, citing confidence in improved electronic tools for detecting inauthentic accounts. It remains to be seen how the platform actions will play out during the 2024 presidential election.

In the legal arena, a Trump-appointed federal judge in Louisiana barred the Biden administration from having any contact with social-media companies and certain research institutions to discuss safeguarding elections in July 2023. The judgement echoed the claims by Jim Jordan and other Republicans that there was collusion between the White House and the social-media companies to censor conservative voices under the guise of fighting disinformation about COVID-19 during the pandemic and false election claims during the 2022 midterms. Although there are important and potentially problematic implications for free speech that arise whenever a government gets involved in managing what it considers misinformation (Neo, 2022 ; Vese, 2022 ), the Louisiana ruling was particularly broad in its prohibitions (West, 2023 ). The implications of the ruling include denying election officials access to information gathered by independent research bodies (the ruling lists “the Election Integrity Partnership, the Virality Project, the Stanford Internet Observatory, or any like project or group”) that would enable them to debunk false election-related information and provide more accurate information instead. The Supreme Court blocked the Louisiana ruling in October 2023 (Hurley, 2023 ) but agreed to a full hearing later in its current term. We return to the conflict between free speech and the adverse effects of disinformation later.

The post-modern critiques of misinformation research

At the heart of research on misinformation is the belief that the concepts of truth and falsehood are essential to democracy, to cognition, and to daily life, and that the status of many, but of course not all claims, can be determined with sufficient accuracy to warrant rebuttal of false information. For example, the “big lie” about a stolen election is just that—it is a lie with no sustainable evidentiary support and it is routinely referred to as such in the scholarly literature (e.g., Arceneaux and Truex, 2022 ; Canon and Sherman, 2021 ; Graham and Yair, 2023 ; Henricksen and Betz, 2023 ; Jacobson, 2021 , 2023 ; Painter and Fernandes, 2022 ). The lie has been rejected by 62 American courts, all of which dismissed or ruled against law suits questioning the legitimacy of the election by Donald Trump or his supporters. Footnote 4

It is curious that the reaction by Trump and some of his most ardent public supporters to such determinative judgments about the falsity of his claims has not been to claim that they are in fact true, but to attack the idea that objective knowledge is even possible. When confronted with a lie, Trump’s adviser Kellyanne Conway once famously quipped that she was presenting “alternative facts.” On another occasion, Trump’s attorney Rudy Giuliani declared that “truth isn’t truth.” Such a strategy seems oddly reminiscent of the postmodernist critique of the possibility of objective knowledge, which first arose as a core aspect of 1930s fascism and was then adapted by left-wing literary criticism from the 1960s onward (Lewandowsky, 2020 ). At that time, humanities scholars had grown increasingly uncomfortable with the idea that facts were just facts, and that there was no role for considering the personal or political interests of those who were engaged in the pursuit of empirical knowledge. In this, postmodernists raised an important point of self-reflection for scientists and others who blithely claimed that there was an impenetrable wall between facts and values. But then they took things too far. Derrida claimed that there was no such thing as objective knowledge. Foucault went on to suggest that —given this— all knowledge claims were nothing more than an assertion of the political interests of the investigator (McIntyre, 2018 , p. 124).

This led to the “science wars” of the 1990s, when scientists and their allies fought back against subjectivism and relativism to defend the importance of objective knowledge at least as a regulative ideal of empirical inquiry. This particular attack on science eventually dissipated —and in the face of damage it had done to objective knowledge claims like the reality of global warming, some postmodernists such as Bruno Latour eventually even apologized (Latour, 2004 )— but the damage was already done. Meanwhile, both the corporate sector and the religious and political right wing had once again taken up the strategy in their attacks on science. The advantage of post-modernism for anti-democratic purposes is obvious, and has echoes of authoritarian attacks on truth-tellers and their defenders throughout history. Indeed, to someone who embraces the idea that their political ideology should have supremacy over objective reality, the advantages of postmodernism are clear. Not only can falsehoods about the economy, crime, and political violence be offered as “alternative narratives” to carefully-measured statistics or other forms of evidence, but the credibility of any party as an objective truth-teller can be undermined. And this suits the authoritarian just fine—for where there is no truth then there can be no blame or accountability either.

Hannah Arendt long ago recognized the dangers of this strategy when she wrote: “the ideal subject of totalitarian rule is not the convinced Nazi or the convinced communist, but people for whom the distinction between fact and fiction … true and false … no longer exist.” This easy political slide into postmodernism does violence to the idea that truth matters, that facts can be discovered through empirical analysis, and that it is crucial to attempt to discern the facts before we can make good policy—especially when we hold competing values that will impact policy choice. And this is true even more so in an era when the creation and amplification of knowledge claims are so easily subject to digital manipulation and weaponization by anyone who has a personal or political interest. Fortunately, researchers have developed conceptual, cognitive, and computational tools that permit the differentiation between legitimate contestation of facts on the one hand, and misinformation and willful disinformation on the other.

The identifiability of contested facts

Notwithstanding our rejection of the postmodernist project, we do not dispute its core idea that many contested assertions cannot be unambiguously adjudicated by referring to “facts”. There are indeed cases in which different actors may legitimately question each other’s “facts”. In our view, these ambiguous cases are precisely those that merit democratic debate and contestation. When conducted in good-faith, such debates can be particularly revealing because both sides can marshal evidence in support.

To illustrate, consider the recent controversy surrounding a machine-learning tool known as COMPAS (Dressel and Farid, 2018 ), which is intended to assist judges in the U.S. by predicting the likelihood of recidivism of a specific offender. Critics accused COMPAS of being racially biased based on statistical analysis of the evidence (Angwin et al., 2016 ). The case rested on the observation that among defendants who ultimately did not re-offend, the algorithm misclassified African-Americans as being at risk of re-offending more than twice as often as White offenders. This misclassification can have serious consequences for a person because judges are inclined to treat high-risk defendants more harshly.

Proponents of COMPAS rejected this charge and argued that the algorithm was not racially biased because it predicted recidivism equally for Black and White offenders for each of its 10 risk categories. That is, the classification into risk categories based on a large number of indicator variables was racially unbiased—a Black person’s actual probability of re-offending was the same as that of a White person with the same risk score (Dieterich et al., 2016 ).

It turns out that it is mathematically impossible to simultaneously satisfy both forms of fairness—calibration and classification—when the base rates of re-offending differ between groups (Berk et al., 2021 ; Lagioia et al., 2023 ). That is, if a greater share of Black people are classified as high-risk—which the algorithm does in an unbiased manner—then it necessarily follows that a greater share of Black defendants who do not re-offend will also be mistakenly classified as high-risk. In those circumstances, it would be inappropriate to accuse one or the other side of spreading misinformation, as each party has mathematical justification for their position and a resolution can only be attained through a value-laden policy discussion. Indeed, to our knowledge, the main contestants in this debate—Northpointe, the manufacturer of COMPAS (Dieterich et al., 2016 ) and ProPublica, a public-interest media organization (Angwin et al., 2016 )—did not level charges of misinformation against each other despite engaging in robust debate.

A similar controversy with even greater stakes arose in the context of the COVID-19 vaccine rollout in the U.S. in 2021. Unlike most other countries, which vaccinated their populations according to age alone—with the elderly being given highest priority because of their much higher mortality rate from COVID-19—the U.S. Advisory Committee on Immunization Practices (ACIP) favored a policy that gave higher priority to essential workers (e.g., food and transport workers) than the elderly. This policy was partially motivated by the fact that racial minorities (Blacks and Hispanics) are underrepresented among adults over 65, whereas they are slightly over-represented among essential workers—thus, under an age-based policy the share of Whites who receive the vaccine would have initially been greater than their proportion in the population would have warranted. Conversely, Blacks would have been underrepresented among the vaccinated early on (Mounk, 2023 ). This inequity could be avoided by first vaccinating essential workers among whom racial minorities were over-represented. However, because the age distribution of essential workers has a much lower average, fewer lives were saved among vaccinated essential workers—whose young age rendered their risk of dying from COVID-19 low to begin with—than would have been saved among the elderly had they been vaccinated (Rumpler et al., 2023 ). Modeling has confirmed that while the essential-worker policy introduced racial equity in terms of doses administered, more lives would have been saved in all ethnic groups under an age-based policy (Rumpler et al., 2023 ). Again, the apparent fairness of a policy depended on the outcome measure: doses administered vs. lives saved. Given the unequal distributions of different ethnic and racial groups across different ages, no mathematical possibility exists to settle on a single “fair” policy. Public opinion appears to have been broadly in line with the policy ultimately adopted by ACIP (Persad et al., 2021 ).

The controversies surrounding COMPAS and ACIP’s vaccination policy are just two instances of a much wider problem, which is that when issues become sufficiently complex, even good-faith actors may find it impossible to agree. One reason is that cognitive limitations prevent a full Bayesian representation (the gold standard of rationality) of the problem (Pothos et al., 2021 ). Instead, people are forced to simplify their representations, for example by partitioning their knowledge (Lewandowsky et al., 2002 ). Persistent and irresolvable disagreements are thus almost ensured by human cognitive limitations (Pothos et al., 2021 ). The second reason is that people differ in their values and weigh evidence differently even if all parties can agree on underlying facts (Walasek and Brown, 2023 ).

Nonetheless, controversies such as those surrounding COMPAS and ACIP’s vaccination policy do not give licence to political actors to obscure the debate through falsehoods, misleading claims, or lies. On the contrary, proper debate of those issues is only possible in the absence of falsehoods because their resolution ultimately requires a trade-off of values that is best arrived at by weighing the importance of different competing sets of evidence. We therefore reject recent academic voices that have questioned whether misinformation can be reliably identified at all (Acerbi et al., 2022 ; Adams et al., 2023 ; Harris, 2022 ; van Doorn, 2023 ; Yee, 2023a , 2023b ). We suggest that its identification is essential and, as we show next, empirically well supported.

The identifiability of misinformation

We place our case into the context of the more extreme end of the academic critique because it involves positions that are antithetical to ours, calling into question the entire idea of fact-checking. For example, Uscinski ( 2015 ) raised the specter that fact-checking is merely a “veiled continuation of politics by means of journalism” (p. 243). Yee ( 2023a ) argued more broadly that any deference to “epistemic elites”—including not only fact-checkers but also academics, researchers, or journalists—is problematic, and assessment of the quality of information should include democratic elements “that are participatory, transparent, and fully negotiable by average citizens” (Yee, 2023a , p. 1111). This demand has several problematic implications. First, it does not explain who counts as “average citizen” and who would belong to the “elite”. At what point should individuals seeking to counter misinformation begin to recuse themselves for fear of accidentally treading on “average” citizens? Is a virologist too “elite” to correct misinformation surrounding the origin of a new virus? What about citizens with a PhD or Master’s degree, how are they being classified? Second, why exactly would one exclude epistemic elites, such as investigative journalists or forensic IT experts, from identifying bad-faith actors such as foreign “bots” or “trolls”? Are average citizens really better at this task than network scientists? Should we decide by social-media poll whether a new strain of avian flu is contagious to humans (Lewandowsky et al., 2017 )? Probably not. There are obviously many domains that benefit from expert assessment of claims.

Nonetheless, there has been much research that has revealed the competence of crowds in the context of fact-checking. For example, Pennycook and Rand ( 2019 ) showed that crowdsourced trust ratings of media outlets were quite successful in the aggregate when compared to ratings by professionals, notwithstanding substantial partisan differences. This basic finding has been replicated and extended several times (M. R. Allen et al., 2024 ; Martel et al., 2024 ), with community-based fact-checking of COVID-19 content being 97% accurate in one study (M. R. Allen et al., 2024 ). Care must, however, be taken that crowds are politically balanced. When people can choose what content to evaluate, as in Twitter/X’s crowdsourced “Birdwatch” fact-checking program (now known as Community Notes), partisan differences among contributors may limit the value of the crowdsourcing (J. Allen et al., 2022 ). The crowdsourcing results show not only that average citizens can match the competence of experts in the aggregate, but they also reaffirm that misinformation is identifiable.

Much recent research has uncovered specific “fingerprints” that can enable people as well as machines to infer the likely quality or accuracy of content. Misinformation has been shown to be suffused with emotions, logical fallacies, and conspiratorial reasoning (Blassnig et al., 2019 ; Carrasco-Farré, 2022 ; Fong et al., 2021 ; Musi et al., 2022 ; Musi and Reed, 2022 ). For example, critical thinking methods offer a qualitative approach to deconstructing arguments in order to identify the presence of reasoning fallacies (Cook et al., 2018 ).

Quantitatively, one study found that compared to reliable information, misinformation is less cognitively complex and 10 times more likely to rely on negative emotional appeals (Carrasco-Farré, 2022 ). In confirmation, numerous other studies show that misinformation is, on average, more emotional than factual information (for a systematic review, see Peng et al., 2023 ) Upward of 75% of anti-vaccination websites use negative emotional appeals (Bean, 2011 ) and linguistic analyses show that conspiracy theorists use significantly more fear-driven language as compared to scientists (Fong et al., 2021 ).

Emotion also plays a role in the receivers’ behavior. People have been shown to be more susceptible to misinformation when put in an emotional state (Martel et al., 2020 ), which helps explain the preferential and more rapid diffusion of unreliable versus reliable information online (Pröllochs et al., 2021 ; Vosoughi et al., 2018 ).

Critics may argue that the datasets used for determining what constitutes “misinformation” and “reliable” information are limited or biased or that the mere prevalence of these cues is not evidence of their diagnosticity in real-world contexts. However, computational machine-learning work relying on a large variety of different URL sources and fact-checked datasets has confirmed that the results are robust and generalizable (Ghanem et al., 2020 ; Kumari et al., 2022 ; Lebernegg et al., 2024 ). A recent comprehensive study which combined many of the available cues found that they have high diagnostic and predictive validity and help discriminate between false and true information, with state-of-the-art models reaching over 83% classification accuracy (Lebernegg et al., 2024 ). Moreover, real-world training on fake news detection, such as logical fallacy training, helps people accurately discriminate between misleading and credible news (e.g., Hruschka and Appel, 2023 ; Lu et al., 2023 ; Roozenbeek et al., 2022 ).

In summary, the available evidence shows quite convincingly that misinformation can be identified by both humans and machines with considerable accuracy. As we show next, we can go beyond mere identification as there are also at least three ways in which one can ascertain the deceptive intent underlying disinformation if present. Identification of deceptive intent is particularly pertinent because it allows information to be safely discounted without requiring a detailed analysis of its factual status.

The identifiability of willful disinformation

For decades, the hallmark of Western news coverage about politicians’ false or misleading claims was an array of circumlocutions that carefully avoided the charge of lying—that is, knowingly telling an untruth with intent to deceive (Lackey, 2013 )—and instead used adverbs such as “falsely”, “wrongly”, “bogus”, or “baseless” when describing a politician’s speech. Other choice phrases referred to “unverified claims” or “repeatedly debunked claims”. This changed in late 2016, when the New York Times first used the word “lie” to characterize an utterance by Donald Trump (Borchers, 2016 ). The paper again referred to Donald Trump’s lies within days of the inauguration in January 2017 (Barry, 2017 ) and it has grown into a routine part of its coverage from then on. Many other mainstream news organizations soon followed suit and it has now become widely accepted practice to refer to Trump’s lies as lies.

Given that lying involves the intentional uttering of false statements, what tools are at our disposal to infer a person’s intention when they utter falsehoods? How can we know a person is lying rather than being confused? How can we infer intentionality?

Anecdotally, defenders of Donald Trump’s lies have raised precisely that objection to the use of the word “lie” in connection with his falsehoods. This objection runs afoul of centuries of legal scholarship and Western jurisprudence. Brown ( 2022 ) argues that inferring intentionality from the evidence is “ordinary and ubiquitous and pervades every area of the law” (p. 2). Inferring intentionality is the difference between manslaughter and murder and is at the heart of the concept of perjury—namely, willfully or knowingly making a false material declaration (Douglis, 2018 ).

There are at least three approaches that can be pursued to infer intentional deception by a communicating agent with varying degrees of confidence. The first approach is statistical and relies on linguistic analysis of material. Unlike people, who are not very good lie detectors despite performing (slightly) above chance (Bond and DePaulo, 2006 ; Mattes et al., 2023 ), recent advances in natural language processing (NLP) have given rise to machine-learning models that can classify texts as deceptive or honest based on subtle linguistic clues (e.g., Braun et al., 2015 ; Davis and Sinnreich, 2020 ; Van Der Zee et al., 2021 ). To illustrate, a model that relied on analysis of the distribution of different types of words achieved 67% accuracy (considerably better than the 52% achieved by human judges) on texts generated by speakers who were either instructed to lie or to be honest. Using the same analysis approach, Davis and Sinnreich ( 2020 ) trained a model to classify tweets by Donald Trump as true or false by using independent fact-checks as ground truth. The model was able to classify tweets with more than 90% accuracy, suggesting that Trump uses subtly different language (e.g., more negative emotion, more prepositions and discrepancies) when communicating untruths. A similar model of Trump’s tweets was developed by Van Der Zee et al. ( 2021 ), who additionally applied 26 extant models from the literature to Trump’s tweets and showed that most of them performed above chance despite being developed on very different materials. In summary, NLP-based approaches have repeatedly shown their value in the classification of speech into honest and deceptive. The fact that those models succeed also when applied to the tweets of Donald Trump implies at the very least that Trump’s falsehoods are not uttered at random or accidentally but are deployed using specific linguistic techniques.

In general, machine-learning approaches to deception detection have shown promise. A recent systematic review identified 81 studies, 19 of which achieved accuracies in excess of 90%, with a further 15 exceeding 80% accuracy (Constâncio et al., 2023 ). The machine-learning models in that ensemble were trained on a variety of corpora, ranging from reviews on Tripadvisor (either true or generated with the intent to deceive; Barsever et al., 2020 ) to segments of a radio game show dedicated to bluff detection by the audience (Papantoniou et al., 2021 ). In all cases, the ground truth (i.e., whether or not deceptive intent was present) was unambiguously known, and the models learned to identify deceptive text based on linguistic analysis with considerable albeit imperfect success.

The second approach to establish willful deception relies on analysis of internal documents of institutions such as governments or corporations. Comparison of the internal knowledge to public stances of the same entities can identify active deception, especially when it is large-scale. Numerous such cases exist, mainly involving corporations and their associated infrastructure such as think-tanks and other front groups (Ceccarelli, 2011 ; Oreskes and Conway, 2010 ). For example, as early as the 1920s, the electricity industry organized a propaganda campaign to falsely insist that private sector electricity was cheaper and more reliable than electricity generated in the public sector (Oreskes and Conway, 2023 ). The tobacco industry’s activities to mislead the public about the dangers from smoking are well documented and established beyond reasonable doubt (e.g., Cataldo et al., 2010 ; Fallin et al., 2013 ; Francey and Chapman, 2000 ; Proctor, 2012 ). The tobacco industry was well aware of the link between smoking and lung cancer in the 1950s and 1960s (Proctor, 2012 ), and yet continued publicly to dispute that medical fact using a variety of propagandistic means (Landman and Glantz, 2009 ; Proctor, 2011 ). Similarly, analysis of internal documents of the fossil-fuel industry has revealed that industry leaders, in particular ExxonMobil, were fully aware of the reality of climate change and its underlying causes (Supran and Oreskes, 2017 , 2021 ) while simultaneously expending large sums to deny its existence in public (J. Farrell, 2016 ) and to prevent Congress from enacting climate-mitigation legislation (Brulle, 2018 ). Ironically, ExxonMobil’s scientists projected global temperatures in the 1970s and 1980s with comparable skill as independent academics at the time (Supran et al., 2023 ). As Baker and Oreskes ( 2017 ) argued, the best explanation for ExxonMobil’s conduct is that they knowingly deceived the public by funding a disinformation machine that denied the realities of climate change. This approach admittedly requires considerable resources and skill, and it is comparatively slow, but in exchange the results it yields are particularly diagnostic and demonstrably useful in litigation. In the case of the tobacco industry, this was the basis for a conviction of Phillip Morris under federal racketeering (RICO) law. The appeals in that case explicitly noted that Phillip Morris intentionally deceived the public and that first-amendment (free speech) rights did not apply as they do not protect fraud or deliberate misrepresentation (Farber et al., 2018 ). In the case of the fossil fuel industry, litigation has not been met with notable success at the time of this writing, but the “Exxon knew” campaign, based on research by Supran and colleagues (Supran et al., 2023 ; Supran and Oreskes, 2017 , 2021 ), has had considerable public impact with 178 relevant media articles identified by Google News. Footnote 5

The final approach to identifying intentional deception resembles the approach involving institutional documents but specifically focuses on lies promulgated by identifiable individuals. We illustrate this approach with Donald Trump’s big lie about the 2020 presidential elections, focusing on statements made in courts of law. Although Trump was making widespread public accusations of fraud, his lawyers—who filed more than 60 lawsuits in connection with the election—did not echo those accusations in court. Quite on the contrary, his lawyers frequently disavowed any mention of fraud in court despite their very different public stance. For example, Rudy Giuliani, one of Trump’s lead attorneys, stood outside a landscaping business on the day most networks declared the election for Biden, and thundered that “It’s [the election] a fraud, an absolute fraud.” Ten days later, being questioned by a federal judge in Pennsylvania during one of Trump’s lawsuits (dealing with whether local election officials in Pennsylvania should have allowed voters to fix problems with their mail-in ballots after submitting them), he declared “This is not a fraud case” (Lerer, 2020 ). This pattern was pervasive: Trump’s lawyers continued to back away from suggestions that the election was stolen and admitted in court that there was no evidence of fraud, all in contradiction to their plaintiff’s public statements (Lerer, 2020 ).

Notwithstanding the careful hedging of their claims in court, the frivolous suits filed on behalf of Trump resulted in sanctions for several of his attorneys. Two lawyers who did claim widespread voter fraud not only had their suit dismissed but were also sanctioned $187,000 by a federal judge in Colorado for their frivolous, meritless case (Polantz, 2021 ). The decision was upheld on appeal, and the Supreme Court declined to hear a further appeal by the lawyers (Scarcella, 2023 ). Altogether, 22 Trump lawyers have been identified who face sanctions in litigation, criminal prosecutions, and state bar disciplinary proceedings. In all cases, what appears to be at issue is violation of the Model Code of Conduct, in particular rules stipulating that claims must be meritorious and that lawyers must exhibit candor and truthfulness (Neff and Fredrickson, 2023 ).

Since the flurry of lawsuits in late 2020, Trump lawyer Sidney Powell has pleaded guilty to charges arising from her involvement in pushing the big lie. Ms Powell pleaded guilty to “conspiracy to commit intentional interference with performance of election duties” and agreed to cooperate with prosecutors in a criminal case against Donald Trump (Fausset and Hakim, 2023 ). Two further Trump lawyers have pleaded guilty in the same case and agreed to testify truthfully about other defendants (Blake, 2023 ).

In a civil suit brought against Rudy Giuliani by two election workers in Georgia, whom he had publicly accused of election fraud, Giuliani conceded before trial that those statements were false (Brumback, 2023 ). The election workers were awarded $148 million in damages, causing Giuliani to file for bankruptcy in late 2023 (Aratani and Oladipo, 2023 ). In a further twist, Giuliani repeated his false claims during the trial outside the court room even while his lawyers conceded in court that they were wrong (Hsu and Weiner, 2023 ).

Giuliani was promptly sued again by the election workers, and at the time of this writing the suit was still under way (Hsu and Weiner, 2023 ).

The big lie was not just curated and pushed by politicians seeking to cling to power and their attorneys. It is now public knowledge that one major news network, Rupert Murdoch’s Fox News, knowingly amplified claims about the election that network executives knew to be false. The fact that Fox lied became apparent during a defamation suit filed by Dominion Voting Machines against the network over false allegations that the voting machines had been rigged to steal the 2020 election. As trial was about to begin, Fox News agreed to pay Dominion $787.5 million and acknowledged that the network had broadcast false statements. The discovery process that preceded trial had uncovered numerous documents and emails that revealed that senior network executives and hosts were convinced that the allegations about the election made by Trump and his allies were untrue (e.g., Peltz, 2023 ; Terkel et al., 2023 ). The network continued to air those allegations and its CEO instructed staff that fact-checking “had to stop” because it was bad for business (Levine, 2023 ). One scholar put it succinctly: “Fox News deliberately misleads the audience for profit” (Nyberg, 2023 , p. 1). Although Fox has been repeatedly implicated in spreading disinformation with harmful consequences for the American public (Ash et al., 2023 ; Bolin and Hamilton, 2018 ; Bursztyn et al., 2020 ; DellaVigna and Kaplan, 2007 ; Feldman et al., 2012 ; Kull et al., 2003 ; Simonov et al., 2022 ), the Dominion case provided a unique opportunity to ascertain that, at least in this case, the network was knowingly lying to its audience.

The preceding examples illustrate the approaches available to establish—with some degree of confidence—the intention to deceive that is the core element of lies. Our examples are not intended to be exhaustive but they illustrate the options available to researchers, journalists, and the public to uncover when they are being lied to. The examples also put to rest several generous auxiliary assumptions that have been made about lies in politics, such as their presumed inevitability because issues can be so nuanced that complete honesty is impossible. Contrary to that assumption, the fact that a person’s rhetoric can differ strikingly between courts of law—where penalties apply for misrepresentations and perjury—and politics—where accountability is notoriously absent—not only reveals the intention to deceive but also the person’s sensitivity to the consequences of their speech.

We have already noted that the contrast between what companies such as ExxonMobil or Philip Morris said in public about their products and what they discussed in private was sufficient to provoke legal consequences. Similar arguments, that fraudulent political speech should not be protected by the First Amendment, have been advanced in the context of Trump’s big lie (Henricksen and Betz, 2023 ).

Although our examination was necessarily limited to a small number of cases, they suffice to illustrate a pathway towards pinpointing intentional disinformation by analysing the utterances of the liars themselves, be they corporations, politicians, or media organizations. We believe that the basic approach is of considerable generality, extending to numerous recorded instances:

Politicians catching themselves lying by changing their story, indicating they were telling an untruth on at least one of those occasions (O’Toole, 2022 , p. 427).

Attorneys of conspiracy theorist Alex Jones—who was sued for his claims that the Sandy Hook massacre never happened by parents of the victims—seeking to defend him by calling him a performance artist who should not be taken seriously (Borchers, 2017 ).

Alex Jones himself admitting in court that the Sandy Hook shooting was “100% real” after having misled millions of people for many years (Associated Press, 2022 ).

Fox News requiring their employees to be vaccinated against COVID-19 or submit to daily testing while the network routinely broadcast anti-vaccination content (Darcy, 2021 ).

Tucker Carlson, former Fox News host, openly admitting that he lies on air (Muzaffar, 2021 ).

Moving forward

Our work explored three fundamental premises: First, that democracy rests on a foundation of common knowledge (H. Farrell and Schneier, 2018 ) and that it is imperiled if citizens cannot agree on basic facts such as the integrity of elections (H. Farrell and Schneier, 2018 ; Tenove, 2020 ). Second, that while democratic debate—including evidence-informed policy-making—often involves contestation of facts (e.g., Kuklinski et al., 1998 ), this does not licence the use of outright lies and propaganda to willfully mislead the public (Lewandowsky, 2020 ). Third, that it is often possible to identify falsehoods, disinformation, and lies and differentiate them from good-faith political and policy-related argumentation.

At the time of this writing, Donald Trump is the Republican nominee for the 2024 presidential election. His campaign has rolled out an explicitly authoritarian agenda for his second term (Arnsdorf and Stein, 2023 ). The authoritarian agenda is likely to result in less free speech, rather than more, which is ironic in light of the fact that people such as Jim Jordan, who are attacking the idea of studying disinformation, do so under the banner of defending the First Amendment. Against this background, the question of how to address Donald Trump’s lies in particular and misinformation in general takes on particular importance.

At the more pessimistic end, Barkho ( 2023 ) posed three questions about the success of fact-checking Trump’s claims: first, have fact-checkers succeeded in persuading Trump to stop disseminating lies? Second, have the long inventories of falsehoods compiled by fact-checkers embarrassed or shamed Trump? Third, has fact-checking changed public perception of what constitutes truth? At first glance, the answer to all three questions might appear to be a resounding “no” (even though the counterfactual is, of course, unknown). However, at the more optimistic end of the spectrum, experimental studies in which election-fraud misinformation was corrected have found positive effects on trust in electoral processes (Bailard et al., 2022 ; Painter and Fernandes, 2022 ), including among Republican respondents and supporters of Trump. Those findings should give rise to a sliver of optimism that even partisans are receptive to corrective messages about election integrity, and therefore underscore the value of disinformation research.

Correcting lies about elections is arguably compatible with the spirit of a democracy. But what is the democratic legitimacy of broader countermeasures against misinformation and disinformation? It is straightforward to explore techniques with which to correct misconceptions in an experiment, in particular if the misinformation is introduced in the experiment itself (e.g., Ecker et al., 2011 ). It is less straightforward to deploy such techniques in the public sphere. Who determines what is “misinformation”, and what is “correct”? And how narrow is the gap between correcting misinformation and banning it? Several countries have recently outlawed “fake news” (e.g., Burkina Faso, Cambodia, Hungary, India, Malaysia, Singapore, and Vietnam) whose democratic credentials are at best questionable. In those cases, fake news can damage democracy not only by disinforming the public but also because countermeasures can be used to curb civil liberties and justify authoritarian crackdowns (Neo, 2022 ; Vese, 2022 ). Indeed, given that Donald Trump has routinely labeled any media coverage he did not like as “fake news”, perhaps the worst response to misinformation would be a law against fake news designed by Donald Trump and his allies.

There are, however, numerous ways in which the public can be better protected by the platforms—in particular if prodded into action by suitable regulations—against disinformation. One avenue involves content moderation and removal of unacceptable or problematic content, such as hate speech. The public is broadly supportive of moderation in certain cases (Kozyreva, Herzog, et al., 2023 ), and the European Union’s recent Digital Services Act (DSA) acknowledges a role for content moderation while highlighting the need for transparency of the underlying rules (for details, see Kozyreva, Smillie, et al., 2023 ). In addition, there are a number of alternative approaches that aim to inform or educate consumers rather than govern content directly. Those approaches have the advantage that they side-step concerns about censorship and that they are demonstrably scalable and readily deployable by the platforms.

One avenue involves the provision of “nutrition labels”, that is, indicators of the quality of a source. Reliable indicators of quality exist that are based on basic journalistic principles (Lin et al., 2023 ), and it is well-known that perceived source credibility can influence misinformation persuasiveness (Nadarevic et al., 2020 ; Prike et al., 2024 ). The effectiveness of source-quality indicators can be enhanced by introducing friction, for example, by requiring users to expend additional clicks to make information visible (L. Fazio, 2020 ; Pillai and Fazio, 2023 ). Naturally, such indicators cannot be perfect, and even sources of widely-acknowledged high quality can also publish dubious content. This makes it important to go beyond credibility and consider alternative approaches, such as those that boost users’ ability to spot deception and enhance their information-discernment skills. This can range from teaching “critical ignoring” (Kozyreva, Wineburg, et al., 2023 ), which enables people to ignore information that is unlikely to warrant expenditure of our limited attention, to psychological inoculation or “prebunking” (Lewandowsky and van der Linden, 2021 ; Roozenbeek et al., 2022 ), which involves refuting a lie in advance by explaining the rhetorical techniques that disinformers use to mislead consumers (e.g., scapegoating, false dichotomies, ad hominem attacks, and so on). Through short “edutainment” videos that are displayed as ads or public-service messages, this approach has been scaled on social media to empower millions of people to spot manipulation techniques (Goldberg, 2023 ). Meta-analyses have affirmed the efficacy of the inoculation approach (Banas and Rains, 2010 ; Lu et al., 2023 ). However, while standard debunking and prebunking interventions promise to be effective regardless of the cultural context in which they are applied (Blair et al., 2024 ; Pereira et al., 2023 ; Porter and Wood, 2021 ; but see Pereira et al., 2022 ), the effects of other interventions such as media-literacy training may be less robust in the Global South (Badrinathan, 2021 ). Some interventions developed and successfully applied in the Global North may also be less suitable in less-developed countries, if for example they target dissemination channels that have limited relevance locally (Badrinathan and Chauchard, 2024 ; de Freitas Melo et al., 2019 ).

Overall, much is now known about various cognitively-inspired countermeasures to correct misinformation or to protect people against being misled in the first place. For further extensive discussion of these countermeasures, see Ecker et al. ( 2022 ) and Kozyreva et al. ( 2024 ). Some of the cognitive science of misinformation has been reflected in European regulatory initiatives, such as the strengthened Code of Practice on Disinformation (Kozyreva, Smillie, et al., 2023 ). In addition, specific evidence-based recommendations for platforms have been developed by Roozenbeek et al. ( 2023 ) and Wardle and Derakhshan ( 2017 ).

Our work has also identified several important questions for future research. We consider the long-term consequences of misinformation on society to be a particularly pressing issue. We have a reasonably good understanding of the individual-level cognitive processes that are engaged when a person is exposed to a single piece of misinformation (Ecker et al., 2022 ). We know very little about the cognitive and social consequences for an individual who is inundated with information of dubious quality for prolonged periods of time. We do not know how societies are affected by epistemic uncertainty and chaos in the long run. Numerous indicators suggest that Western societies, in particular the United States, are ailing (e.g., Lewandowsky et al., 2017 ), but the attribution of those trends to misinformation or epistemic chaos is difficult. On those occasions where researchers have successfully isolated causal effects, they tend to implicate certain media organs (e.g., Fox News in particular) in compromising public health (Bursztyn et al., 2020 ; Simonov et al., 2020 ), and they have identified the role of social media in causing ethnic hate crimes and xenophobia (Bursztyn et al., 2019 ; Müller and Schwarz, 2021 ). However, it is unclear as yet how generalizable those findings are and much additional work remains to be done (for a review, see Lorenz-Spreen et al., 2022 ).

Future research should also address some of the limitations of fact-checking, such as the difficulties of verifying statements about the future (Nieminen and Sankari, 2021 ) or arguments that employ the rhetorical technique of “paltering” — that is, the use of truthful statements to convey a misleading impression (Lewandowsky et al., 2016 ; Rogers et al., 2017 ). One approach is to focus on what is pragmatically useful for people to make informed decisions, such as whether a claim is misleading (Birks, 2019 ), with critical thinking methods offering a means of identifying the presence of logical fallacies (Cook et al., 2018 ).

Increasing research attention is being paid to the concept of discernment; that is, the extent to which accurate misinformation is believed more than misinformation (Pennycook and Rand, 2021 ). Focusing on discernment rather than acceptance of misinformation guards against inadvertently developing interventions that reduce belief in facts and misinformation equally. A general cynicism and disbelief of everything does not solve the misinformation problem. Instead, we must boost people’s ability to distinguish between facts and falsehoods.

We began the paper with a quote from Hannah Arendt, one of the foremost analysts of 20th century totalitarianism. It is worth here revisiting the same quotation in its extended form, which underscores the urgency of finding a solution to the epistemic crisis affecting democracy in the U.S. and beyond:

“If everybody always lies to you, the consequence is not that you believe the lies, but rather that nobody believes anything any longer…. And a people that no longer can believe anything cannot make up its mind. It is deprived not only of its capacity to act but also of its capacity to think and to judge. And with such a people you can then do what you please .” (our emphasis)

Further detailed debunkings of election disinformation are provided by the Cybersecurity and Infrastructure Security Agency at https://www.cisa.gov/topics/election-security/rumor-vs-reality .

We focus here on the activities of Jim Jordan because he is the acknowledged leader of a political counter movement aimed at misinformation research. This must not be taken to imply that Jordan is the only political actor involved in this effort.

https://www.nationalacademies.org/documents/embed/link/LF2255DA3DD1C41C0A42D3BEF0989ACAECE3053A6A9B/file/DC4CDD2AC5D4B2DB08255A7EA6244AA9D7CA6F951C22?noSaveAs=1

One ruling that was initially in Trump’s favor was later overturned by the Pennsylvania Supreme Court. Canon and Sherman ( 2021 ) provide a list of cases.

Search conducted on 10 April 2024.

Acerbi A, Altay S, Mercier H (2022) Research note: Fighting misinformation or fighting for information? Harv Kennedy School Misinform Rev 3. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-87

Adams Z, Osman M, Bechlivanidis C, Meder B (2023) (Why) Is Misinformation a Problem? Perspect Psychol Sci 17456916221141344. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221141344

Agiesta J, Edwards-Levy A (2023) CNN poll: Percentage of Republicans who think Biden’s 2020 win was illegitimate ticks back up near 70%. CNN. https://edition.cnn.com/2023/08/03/politics/cnn-poll-republicans-think-2020-election-illegitimate/index.html

Allen J, Martel C, Rand DG (2022) Birds of a feather don’t fact-check each other: Partisanship and the evaluation of news in Twitter’s Birdwatch crowdsourced fact-checking program. CHI Conf Hum Factors Comp Syst 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1145/3491102.3502040

Allen MR, Desai N, Namazi A, Leas E, Dredze M, Smith DM, Ayers JW (2024) Characteristics of X (formerly Twitter) Community Notes addressing COVID-19 vaccine misinformation. JAMA 331:1670. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2024.4800

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Angwin J, Larson J, Mattu S, Kirchner L (2016) Machine bias: There’s software used across the country to predict future criminals and it’s biased against blacks. ProPublica. https://www.propublica.org/article/machine-bias-risk-assessments-in-criminal-sentencing

Aratani L, Oladipo G (2023) Giuliani files for bankruptcy after judge rules Georgia election workers can collect $148m. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2023/dec/21/giuliani-148-million-damages-georgia-lawsuit

Arceneaux K, Truex R (2022) Donald Trump and the Lie. Perspect Polit 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592722000901

Arnsdorf I, Stein J (2023) Trump touts authoritarian vision for second term: ‘I am your justice’. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/elections/2023/04/21/trump-agenda-policies-2024/

Arun C (2019) On WhatsApp, rumours, and lynchings. Econ Polit Wkly 54(6):30–35

Google Scholar  

Ash E, Galletta S, Hangartner D, Margalit Y, Pinna M (2023) The effect of Fox News on health behavior during COVID-19. Polit Anal 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1017/pan.2023.21

Associated Press (2022) Alex Jones concedes that the Sandy Hook attack was ’100% real’. NPR. https://www.npr.org/2022/08/03/1115414563/alex-jones-sandy-hook-case

Badrinathan S (2021) Educative interventions to combat misinformation: evidence from a field experiment in India. Am Polit Sci Rev 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055421000459

Badrinathan S, Chauchard S (2024) Researching and countering misinformation in the Global South. Curr Opin Psychol 55:101733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101733

Bailard, CS, Porter, E, & Gross, K (2022). Fact-checking Trump’s election lies can improve confidence in U.S. elections: Experimental evidence. Harv Kennedy School Misinform Rev. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-109

Baker E, Oreskes N (2017) Science as a game, marketplace or both: a reply to Steve Fuller. Soc Epistemol Rev Reply Collect 6:65–69

Banas JA, Rains SA (2010) A meta-analysis of research on inoculation theory. Commun Monogr 77:281–311

Article   Google Scholar  

Barkho L (2023) A critical inquiry into US media’s fact-checking and compendiums of Donald Trump’s falsehoods and “lies”. In A Akande (Ed.) The perils of populism: The end of the American century? (pp. 259–278). Springer

Barrett PM, Sims JG (2021) False accusation: The unfounded claim that social media companies censor conservatives (tech. rep.). New York University Stern Center for Business and Human Rights

Barry D (2017). In a swirl of ‘untruths’ and ‘falsehoods,’ calling a lie a lie. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/01/25/business/media/donald-trump-lie-media.html

Barsever D, Singh S, Neftci E (2020) Building a better lie detector with BERT: The difference between truth and lies. 2020 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN). https://doi.org/10.1109/ijcnn48605.2020.9206937

Bean SJ (2011) Emerging and continuing trends in vaccine opposition website content. Vaccine 29:1874–1880. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2011.01.003

Benkler Y, Faris R, Roberts H (2018) Network propaganda: Manipulation, disinformation, and radicalization in American politics. Oxford University Press

Berk R, Heidari H, Jabbari S, Kearns M, Roth A (2021) Fairness in criminal justice risk assessments: The state of the art. Sociol Methods Res 50:3–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124118782533

Article   MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Berlinski N, Doyle M, Guess AM, Levy G, Lyons B, Montgomery JM, Nyhan B, Reifler J (2021) The effects of unsubstantiated claims of voter fraud on confidence in elections. J Exp Polit Sci 10(1), 34–49. https://doi.org/10.1017/xps.2021.18

Bernstein A (2023) Republican Rep. Jim Jordan issues sweeping information requests to universities researching disinformation. Pro Publica. https://www.propublica.org/article/jim-jordan-disinformation-subpoena-universities

Birks J (2019) Fact-checking journalism and political argumentation: A British perspective. Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-30573-4

Blair RA, Gottlieb J, Nyhan B, Paler L, Argote P, Stainfield CJ (2024) Interventions to counter misinformation: Lessons from the Global North and applications to the Global South. Curr Opin Psychol 55:101732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101732

Blake A (2023) Jenna Ellis’s tearful guilty plea should worry Rudy Giuliani. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/politics/2023/10/24/jenna-ellis-guilty-plea-georgia-giuliani-trump/

Blassnig S, Büchel F, Ernst N, Engesser S (2019) Populism and informal fallacies: an analysis of right-wing populist rhetoric in election campaigns. Argumentation 33:107–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-018-9461-2

Blitzer J (2023) Jim Jordan’s conspiratorial quest for power. The New Yorker. https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2023/10/30/jim-jordans-conspiratorial-quest-for-power

Bolin JL, Hamilton LC (2018) The news you choose: News media preferences amplify views on climate change. Environ Polit. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2018.1423909

Bond CF, DePaulo BM (2006) Accuracy of deception judgments. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 10:214–234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2

Borchers C (2016) Why the New York Times decided it is now okay to call Donald Trump a liar. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-fix/wp/2016/09/22/why-the-new-york-times-decided-it-is-now-okay-to-call-donald-trump-a-liar/

Borchers C (2017) Alex Jones should not be taken seriously, according to Alex Jones’s lawyers. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-fix/wp/2017/04/17/trump-called-alex-jones-amazing-joness-own-lawyer-calls-him-a-performance-artist/

Braun MT, Swol LMV, Vang L (2015) His lips are moving: Pinocchio effect and other lexical indicators of political deceptions. Discourse Process 52:1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.942833

Article   ADS   Google Scholar  

Brown TR (2022) Demystifying mindreading for the law. Wisconsin Law Review Forward, 1–11

Brulle RJ (2018) The climate lobby: A sectoral analysis of lobbying spending on climate change in the USA, 2000 to 2016. Climatic Change. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-018-2241-z

Brumback K (2023) Giuliani concedes he made public comments falsely claiming Georgia election workers committed fraud. Associated Press. https://apnews.com/article/giuliani-georgia-election-workers-lawsuit-false-statements-afc64a565ee778c6914a1a69dc756064

Bursztyn L, Egorov G, Enikolopov R, Petrova M (2019) Social media and xenophobia: Evidence from Russia (tech. rep.). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w26567

Bursztyn L, Rao A, Roth C, Yanagizawa-Drott D (2020) Misinformation during a pandemic (tech. rep.). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27417

Canon DT, Sherman O (2021) Debunking the “Big Lie”: Election Administration in the 2020 Presidential Election. Pres Stud Q 51:546–581. https://doi.org/10.1111/psq.12721

Carrasco-Farré C (2022) The fingerprints of misinformation: How deceptive content differs from reliable sources in terms of cognitive effort and appeal to emotions. Hum Soc Sci Commun 9:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01174-9

Cataldo JK, Bero LA, Malone RE (2010) “A delicate diplomatic situation”: Tobacco industry efforts to gain control of the Framingham study. J Clin Epidemiol 63:841–853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.01.021

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Ceccarelli L (2011) Manufactured scientific controversy: Science, rhetoric, and public debate. Rhetor Public Aff 14:195–228

Constâncio AS, Tsunoda DF, Silva HDFN, Silveira JMD, Carvalho DR (2023) Deception detection with machine learning: a systematic review and statistical analysis. PLoS One 18:e0281323. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0281323

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Cook J, Ellerton P, Kinkead D (2018) Deconstructing climate misinformation to identify reasoning errors. Environ Res Lett 13:024018

Cooley A, Nexon DH (2022) The real crisis of global order: Illiberalism on the rise. Foreign Aff 101:103–118

Darcy O (2021) Fox has quietly implemented its own version of a vaccine passport while its top personalities attack them. CNN. https://edition.cnn.com/2021/07/19/media/fox-vaccine-passport/index.html

Davis D, Sinnreich A (2020) Beyond fact-checking: Lexical patterns as lie detectors in Donald Trump’s tweets. Int J Commun 14:5237–5260

de Freitas Melo P, Vieira CC, Garimella K, de Melo POV, Benevenuto F (2019) Can WhatsApp counter misinformation by limiting message forwarding? International Conference on Complex Networks and Their Applications, 372–384. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-36687-2_31

DellaVigna S, Kaplan E (2007) The fox news effect: media bias and voting. Q J Econ 122:1187–1234

Desikan A, MacKinney T, Kalman C, Carter JM, Reed G, Goldman GT (2023) An equity and environmental justice assessment of anti-science actions during the Trump administration. J Public Health Policy 44:147–162. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41271-022-00390-6

Dieterich W, Mendoza C, Brennan T (2016) COMPAS risk scales: Demonstrating accuracy equity and predictive parity. (tech. rep.). Northpoint, Inc

Dixit P, Mac R (2018) How WhatsApp destroyed a village. BuzzFeed News. https://www.buzzfeednews.com/article/pranavdixit/whatsapp-destroyed-village-lynchings-rainpada-india

Douglis A (2018) Disentangling perjury and lying. Yale J Law Hum 29:339–374

Dourado T, Salgado S (2021) Disinformation in the Brazilian pre-election context: Probing the content, spread and implications of fake news about Lula da Silva. Commun Rev 24:297–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/10714421.2021.1981705

Dressel J, Farid H (2018) The accuracy, fairness, and limits of predicting recidivism. Sci Adv 4:eaao5580

Article   ADS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, Apai J (2011) Terrorists brought down the plane!—No, actually it was a technical fault: Processing corrections of emotive information. Q J Exp Psychol 64:283–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.497927

Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, Cook J, Schmid P, Fazio LK, Brashier N, Kendeou P, Vraga EK, Amazeen MA (2022) The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nat Rev Psychol 1:13–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y

Eggers AC, Garro H, Grimmer J (2021) No evidence for systematic voter fraud: a guide to statistical claims about the 2020 election. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118:e2103619118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2103619118

Enders A, Farhart C, Miller J, Uscinski J, Saunders K, Drochon H (2022) Are republicans and conservatives more likely to believe conspiracy theories? Polit Behav 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09812-3

Enders AM, Uscinski JE (2021) Are misinformation, antiscientific claims, and conspiracy theories for political extremists? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

Fallin A, Grana R, Glantz SA (2013) ‘to quarterback behind the scenes, third-party efforts’: the tobacco industry and the tea party. Tob Control 0:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050815

Farber HJ, Neptune ER, Ewart GW (2018) Corrective statements from the tobacco industry: more evidence for why we need effective tobacco control. Ann Am Thorac Soc 15:127–130. https://doi.org/10.1513/annalsats.201711-845gh

Farrell H, Schneier B (2018) Common-knowledge attacks on democracy (tech. rep.). Berkman Klein Center for Internet & Society

Farrell J (2016) Network structure and influence of the climate change counter-movement. Nat Clim Change 6:370–374. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2875

Fausset R, Hakim D (2023) Sidney Powell pleads guilty in Georgia Trump case. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2023/10/19/us/sidney-powell-guilty-plea-trump-georgia.html

Fazio LK, Brashier NM, Payne BK, Marsh EJ (2015) Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. J Exp Psychol General. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000098

Fazio L (2020) Pausing to consider why a headline is true or false can help reduce the sharing of false news. Harv Kennedy School Misinform Rev. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-009

Feldman L, Maibach EW, Roser-Renouf C, Leiserowitz A (2012) Climate on cable: the nature and impact of global warming coverage on Fox News, CNN, and MSNBC. Int J Press/Polit 17:3–31

Field H, Vanian J (2023) Tech layoffs ravage the teams that fight online misinformation and hate speech. CNBC. https://www.cnbc.com/2023/05/26/tech-companies-are-laying-off-their-ethics-and-safety-teams-.html

Fong A, Roozenbeek J, Goldwert D, Rathje S, van der Linden S (2021) The language of conspiracy: a psychological analysis of speech used by conspiracy theorists and their followers on Twitter. Group Process Intergroup Relat 24:606–623. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430220987596

Francey N, Chapman S (2000) “operation Berkshire”: the international tobacco companies’ conspiracy. Br Med J 321:371–374. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.321.7257.371

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Garrett RK, Bond RM (2021) Conservatives’ susceptibility to political misperceptions. Sci Adv 7(23):eabf1234. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abf1234

Ghanem B, Rosso P, Rangel F (2020) An emotional analysis of false information in social media and news articles. ACM Trans Internet Technol 20:19:1–19:18. https://doi.org/10.1145/3381750

Goldberg B (2023) Defanging disinformation’s threat to Ukrainian refugees. Jigsaw. https://medium.com/jigsaw/defanging-disinformations-threat-to-ukrainian-refugees-b164dbbc1c60

González-Bailón S, Lazer D, Barberá P, Zhang M, Allcott H, Brown T, Crespo-Tenorio A, Freelon D, Gentzkow M, Guess AM, Iyengar S, Kim YM, Malhotra N, Moehler D, Nyhan B, Pan J, Rivera CV, Settle J, Thorson E, Tucker JA (2023) Asymmetric ideological segregation in exposure to political news on Facebook. Science 381:392–398. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.ade7138

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Graham MH, Yair O (2023) Expressive responding and trump’s big lie. Polit Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-023-09875-w

Greene KT (2024) Partisan differences in the sharing of low-quality news sources by U.S. political elites. Polit Commun 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2024.2306214

Grice HP (1975) Logic and conversation. In P Cole & JL Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 3: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). Academic Press

Grinberg N, Joseph K, Friedland L, Swire-Thompson B, Lazer D (2019) Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Science 363:374–378. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau2706

Grofman B, Cervas J (2023) Statistical fallacies in claims about ‘massive and widespread fraud’ in the 2020 presidential election: examining claims based on aggregate election results 1,2. Stat Public Policy 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/2330443X.2023.2289529

Guess AM, Nyhan B, Reifler J (2020a) Exposure to untrustworthy websites in the 2016 U.S. election. Nat Hum Behav 4:472–480. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0833-x

Guess AM, Lockett D, Lyons B, Montgomery JM, Nyhan B, Reifler J (2020b) “Fake news” may have limited effects on political participation beyond increasing beliefs in false claims. Harv Kennedy School Misinform Rev 1(1). https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-004

Guess AM, Nagler J, Tucker J (2019) Less than you think: prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on Facebook. Sci Adv 5:eaau4586. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aau4586

Harris KR (2022) Real fakes: The epistemology of online misinformation. Philos Technol 35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-022-00581-9

Henricksen W, Betz B (2023) The stolen election lie and the freedom of speech. Penn State Law Review. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4354211

Hotez P (2023) Anti-science conspiracies pose new threats to US biomedicine in 2023. FASEB BioAdvances. https://doi.org/10.1096/fba.2023-00032

Hruschka TMJ, Appel M (2023) Learning about informal fallacies and the detection of fake news: An experimental intervention. PLoS One 18:e0283238

Hsu SS, Weiner R (2023) Defamed Georgia poll workers who won $148M from Giuliani sue him again. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/dc-md-va/2023/12/18/giuliani-defamation-lawsuit-georgia/

Hurley L (2023) Supreme Court blocks restrictions on Biden administration efforts to get platforms to remove social media posts. NBC News. https://www.nbcnews.com/politics/supreme-court/supreme-court-blocks-biden-social-media-curbs-rcna105785

Huszár F, Ktena SI, O’Brien C, Belli L, Schlaikjer A, Hardt M (2022) Algorithmic amplification of politics on Twitter. Proc Natl Acad Sci 119:e2025334119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2025334119

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Jacobson GC (2021) Donald Trump’s big lie and the future of the republican party. Pres Stud Q 51:273–289. https://doi.org/10.1111/psq.12716

Jacobson GC (2023) The dimensions, origins, and consequences of belief in Donald Trump’s Big Lie. Polit Sci Q 138:133–166. https://doi.org/10.1093/psquar/qqac030

Jalli N, Idris I (2019) Fake news and elections in two Southeast Asian nations: A comparative study of Malaysia general election 2018 and Indonesia presidential election 2019. Proceedings of the International Conference of Democratisation in Southeast Asia (ICDeSA 2019). https://doi.org/10.2991/icdesa-19.2019.30

Jung Y, Lee S (2023) Trump vs. the GOP: Political Determinants of COVID-19 Vaccination. Polit Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-023-09882-x

Kellow CL, Steeves HL (1998) The role of radio in the Rwandan genocide. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1998.tb02762.x

Kinser S (2020) Science in an age of scrutiny: How scientists can respond to criticism and personal attacks. Union of Concerned Scientists. https://www.ucsusa.org/sites/default/files/2020-09/science-in-an-age-of-scrutiny-2020.pdf

Kozyreva A, Herzog SM, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R, Lorenz-Spreen P, Leiser M, Reifler J (2023) Resolving content moderation dilemmas between free speech and harmful misinformation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 120:e2210666120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2210666120

Kozyreva A, Lorenz-Spreen P, Herzog SM, Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R, Ali A, Bak-Coleman JB, Barzilai S, Basol M, Berinsky A, Betsch C, Cook J, Fazio LK, Geers M, Guess AM, Huang H, Larreguy H, Maertens R, … Wineburg S (2024) Toolbox of interventions against online misinformation. Nat Hum Behav. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x8ejt

Kozyreva A, Smillie L, Lewandowsky S (2023) Incorporating psychological science into policy making. Eur Psychol 28:206–224. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000493

Kozyreva A, Wineburg S, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R (2023) Critical ignoring as a core competence for digital citizens. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 32:81–88. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214221121570

Kuklinski JH, Quirk PJ, Schwieder DW, Rich RF (1998) “Just the facts, ma’am”: political facts and public opinion. Ann Am Acad Political Soc Sci 560:143–154. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716298560001011

Kull S, Ramsay C, Lewis E (2003) Misperceptions, the media, and the Iraq war. Political Sci Q 118:569–598

Kumari R, Ashok N, Ghosal T, Ekbal A (2022) What the fake? Probing misinformation detection standing on the shoulder of novelty and emotion. Inf Process Manag 59:102740. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2021.102740

Lackey J (2013) Lies and deception: an unhappy divorce. Analysis. https://doi.org/10.1093/analys/ant006

Lagioia F, Rovatti R, Sartor G (2023) Algorithmic fairness through group parities? The case of COMPAS-SAPMOC. AI & SOCIETY, 38, 459–478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-022-01441-y

Landman A, Glantz SA (2009) Tobacco industry efforts to undermine policy-relevant research. Am J Public Health 99:45–58. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2004.050963

Lasser J, Aroyehun ST, Simchon A, Carrella F, Garcia D, Lewandowsky S (2022) Social media sharing of low quality news sources by political elites. PNAS Nexus, pgac186. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac186

Latour B (2004) Why has critique run out of steam? From matters of fact to matters of concern. Crit Inq 30:225–248

Lebernegg N, Eberl J-M, Tolochko P, Boomgaarden H (2024) Do you speak disinformation? Computational detection of deceptive news-like content using linguistic and stylistic features. Digit J. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2024.2305792

Leonhardt D (2021) Red Covid. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2021/09/27/briefing/covid-red-states-vaccinations.html

Lerer L (2020) Giuliani in public: ‘it’s a fraud.’ Giuliani in court: ‘This is not a fraud case.’ New York Times https://www.nytimes.com/2020/11/18/us/politics/trump-giuliani-voter-fraud.html

Levine S (2023) Angry Fox News chief said fact-checks of Trump’s election lies ‘bad for business’. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/media/2023/mar/29/fox-news-trump-fact-check-election-lies-dominion

Lewandowsky S (2020) Willful construction of ignorance: A tale of two ontologies. In R Hertwig & C Engel (Eds.), Deliberate ignorance: Choosing not to know (pp. 101–117). MIT Press

Lewandowsky S, Ballard T, Oberauer K, Benestad R (2016) A blind expert test of contrarian claims about climate data. Glob Environ Change 39:91–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.04.013

Lewandowsky S, Ecker UKH, Cook J (2017) Beyond misinformation: understanding and coping with the post-truth era. J Appl Res Mem Cogn 6:353–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.07.008

Lewandowsky S, Kalish ML, Ngang S (2002) Simplified learning in complex situations: Knowledge partitioning in function learning. J Exp Psychol Gen 131:163–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.2.163

Lewandowsky S, Robertson RE, DiResta R (2023a) Challenges in understanding human-algorithm entanglement during online information consumption. Perspect Psychol Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231180809

Lewandowsky S, Stritzke WGK, Freund AM, Oberauer K, Krueger JI (2013) Misinformation, disinformation, and violent conflict: From Iraq and the “War on Terror” to future threats to peace. Am Psychol 68:487–501. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034515

Lewandowsky S (2022) Fake news and participatory propaganda. In R Pohl (Ed.), Cogn illusions (pp. 324–340). Routledge https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003154730-23

Lewandowsky S, Ecker UKH, Cook J, van der Linden S, Roozenbeek J, Oreskes N (2023b) Misinformation and the epistemic integrity of democracy. Curr Opin Psychol 101711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101711

Lewandowsky S, Pomerantsev P (2022) Technology and democracy: a paradox wrapped in a contradiction inside an irony. Memory Mind Media 1. https://doi.org/10.1017/mem.2021.7

Lewandowsky S, van der Linden S (2021) Countering misinformation and fake news through inoculation and prebunking. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 32:348–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2021.1876983

Li D (2004) Echoes of violence: considerations on radio and genocide in Rwanda. J Genocide Res 6:9–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/1462352042000194683

Lin H, Lasser J, Lewandowsky S, Cole R, Gully A, Rand DG, Pennycook G (2023) High level of correspondence across different news domain quality rating sets. PNAS Nexus 2:pgad286. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad286

Lorenz-Spreen P, Oswald L, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R (2022) A systematic review of worldwide causal and correlational evidence on digital media and democracy. Nat Hum Behav 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01460-1

Lu C, Hu B, Li Q, Bi C, Ju X-D (2023) Psychological inoculation for credibility assessment, sharing intention, and discernment of misinformation: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res 25:e49255. https://doi.org/10.2196/49255

Martel C, Allen J, Pennycook G, Rand DG (2024) Crowds can effectively identify misinformation at scale. Perspect Psychol Sci 19:477–488. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231190388

Martel C, Pennycook G, Rand DG (2020) Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cogn Res Princ Implic 5:47. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-020-00252-3

Mattes K, Popova V, Evans JR (2023) Deception detection in politics: can voters tell when politicians are lying. Polit Behav 45:395–418. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09747-1

McGraw KM (1998) Manipulating public opinion with moral justification. Ann Am Acad Political Soc Sci 560:129–142. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716298560001010

McIntyre L (2018) Post-truth. MIT Press

McLauchlin T (2023) Tail risks for 2024: Prospects for a violent constitutional crisis in the United States (tech. rep. No. 28). Network for Strategic Analysis, Queen’s University, Canada

Mounk Y (2023) The identity trap. Penguin Random House

Müller K, Schwarz C (2021) Fanning the flames of hate: social media and hate crime. J Eur Econ Assoc 19:2131–2167. https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvaa045

Musi E, Aloumpi M, Carmi E, Yates S, O’Halloran K (2022) Developing fake news immunity: Fallacies as misinformation triggers during the pandemic. Online J Commun Media Technol 12:e202217. https://doi.org/10.30935/ojcmt/12083

Musi E, Reed C (2022) From fallacies to semi-fake news: improving the identification of misinformation triggers across digital media. Discourse Soc 33:349–370. https://doi.org/10.1177/09579265221076609

Muzaffar M (2021) Tucker Carlson admits he lies on his show: ‘I really try not to… [but] I certainly do’. The Independent. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/world/americas/tucker-carlson-fox-news-dave-rubin-b1919738.html

Nadarevic L, Reber R, Helmecke AJ, Köse D (2020) Perceived truth of statements and simulated social media postings: An experimental investigation of source credibility, repeated exposure, and presentation format. Cogn Res Princ Implic 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-020-00251-4

Nan X, Wang Y, Thier K (2022) Why do people believe health misinformation and who is at risk? A systematic review of individual differences in susceptibility to health misinformation. Soc Sci Med 314:115398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115398

Neff A, Fredrickson C (2023) Trump’s lawyers face sanctions, discipline, and indictment – how should the legal profession respond? Just Security. https://www.justsecurity.org/90509/trumps-lawyers-face-sanctions-discipline-and-indictment-how-should-the-legal-profession-respond/

Neo R (2022) A cudgel of repression: analysing state instrumentalisation of the ‘fake news’ label in Southeast Asia. Journalism 23:1919–1938. https://doi.org/10.1177/1464884920984060

Nieminen S, Sankari V (2021) Checking PolitiFact’s fact-checks. J Stud 22:358–378. https://doi.org/10.1080/1461670x.2021.1873818

Nix N, Menn J (2023) These academics studied falsehoods spread by Trump. Now the GOP wants answers. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/technology/2023/06/06/disinformation-researchers-congress-jim-jordan/

Nix N, Zakrzewski C, Menn J (2023) Misinformation research isbuckling under GOP legal attacks. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/technology/2023/09/23/online-misinformation-jim-jordan/

Nyberg D (2023) The passive revolution is televised: The dominant ideology of media capitalism. Organization, 13505084231180288. https://doi.org/10.1177/13505084231180288

Ognyanova K, Lazer D, Robertson RE, Wilson C (2020) Misinformation in action: Fake news exposure is linked to lower trust in media, higher trust in government when your side is in power. Harv Kennedy School Misinform Rev. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-024

Oreskes N, Conway EM (2010) Merchants of doubt. Bloomsbury Publishing

Oreskes N, & Conway EM (2023) The big myth. New York City: Bloomsbury Publishing

O’Toole F (2022) We don’t know ourselves: A personal history of Ireland since 1958. Head of Zeus

Painter DL, Fernandes J (2022) “the big lie”: How fact checking influences support for insurrection. Am Behav Sci 000276422211031. https://doi.org/10.1177/00027642221103179

Papantoniou K, Papadakos P, Patkos T, Flouris G, Androutsopoulos I, Plexousakis D (2021) Deception detection in text and its relation to the cultural dimension of individualism/collectivism. Nat Lang Eng 28:545–606. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1351324921000152

Peltz M (2023) New details in Dominion suit reveal damning evidence of deception in Fox News’ 2020 election coverage. Mediamatters. https://www.mediamatters.org/foxdominion-lawsuit/new-details-dominion-suit-reveal-damning-evidence-deception-fox-news-2020

Peng W, Lim S, Meng J (2023) Persuasive strategies in online health misinformation: a systematic review. Inf Commun Soc 26:2131–2148. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2022.2085615

Pennycook G, Rand DG (2021) Research note: Examining false beliefs about voter fraud in the wake of the 2020 presidential election. Harvard Kennedy School (HKS) Misinform Rev 2 . https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-51

Pennycook G, Rand DG (2019) Fighting misinformation on social media using crowdsourced judgments of news source quality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1806781116

Pereira FB, Bueno NS, Nunes F, Pavão N (2022) Fake news, fact checking, and partisanship: the resilience of rumors in the 2018 brazilian elections. J Polit 84:2188–2201. https://doi.org/10.1086/719419

Pereira FB, Bueno NS, Nunes F, Pavão N (2023) Inoculation reduces misinformation: experimental evidence from multidimensional interventions in brazil. J Exp Polit Sci 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1017/xps.2023.11

Persad G, Emanuel EJ, Sangenito S, Glickman A, Phillips S, Largent EA (2021) Public perspectives on COVID-19 vaccine prioritization. JAMA Netw Open 4:e217943. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.7943

Pillai RM, Fazio LK (2023) Explaining why headlines are true or false reduces intentions to share false information. Collabra: Psychol 9. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87617

Pinna M, Picard L, Goessmann C (2022) Cable news and COVID-19 vaccine uptake. Sci Rep 12:16804. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20350-0

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Polantz K (2021) Lawyers sanctioned for ‘conspiracy theory’ election fraud lawsuit. CNN. https://edition.cnn.com/2021/08/04/politics/lawyers-colorado-2020-election/index.html

Porter E, Wood TJ (2021) The global effectiveness of fact-checking: evidence from simultaneous experiments in Argentina, Nigeria, South Africa, and the United Kingdom. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118:e2104235118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2104235118

Pothos EM, Lewandowsky S, Basieva I, Barque-Duran A, Tapper K, Khrennikov A (2021) Information overload for (bounded) rational agents. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 288:20202957. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.2957

Prike T, Butler LH, Ecker UKH (2024) Source-credibility information and social norms improve truth discernment and reduce engagement with misinformation online. Sci Rep 14:6900. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57560-7

Proctor RN (2011) Golden holocaust: Origins of the cigarette catastrophe and the case for abolition. University of California Press

Proctor RN (2012) The history of the discovery of the cigarette–lung cancer link: evidentiary traditions, corporate denial, global toll. Tob Control 21(2):87–91. https://doi.org/10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2011-050338

Pröllochs N, Bär D, Feuerriegel S (2021) Emotions explain differences in the diffusion of true vs. false social media rumors. Sci Rep 11:22721. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-01813-2

Reid T (2022) Voting machine problems in Arizona seized on by Trump, election deniers. Reuters. https://www.reuters.com/world/us/voting-machine-problems-battleground-arizona-seized-by-trump-election-deniers-2022-11-08/

Rogers T, Zeckhauser R, Gino F, Norton MI, Schweitzer ME (2017) Artful paltering: the risks and rewards of using truthful statements to mislead others. J Personal Soc Psychol 112:456–473

Roozenbeek J, van der Linden S, Goldberg B, Rathje S, Lewandowsky S (2022) Psychological inoculation improves resilience against misinformation on social media. Sci Adv 8:eabo6254. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abo6254

Roozenbeek J, Suiter J, Culloty E (2023) Countering misinformation: evidence, knowledge gaps, and implications of current interventions. Eur Psychol. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/b52um

Rumpler E, Feldman JM, Bassett MT, Lipsitch M (2023) Fairness and efficiency considerations in COVID-19 vaccine allocation strategies: A case study comparing front-line workers and 65–74 year olds in the United States. PLOS Glob Public Health 3:e0001378. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0001378

Rutenberg J, Myers SL (2024) How Trump’s allies are winning the war over disinformation. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2024/03/17/us/politics/trump-disinformation-2024-social-media.html

Salek TA (2023) Deflecting deliberation through rhetorical nihilism: “Stop the Steal” as an unethical and intransigent rival public. Commun Democracy 57:94–118. https://doi.org/10.1080/27671127.2023.2202744

Scarcella M (2023) US Supreme Court rebuffs lawyers punished after ‘woeful’ suit backing Trump. Reuters. https://www.reuters.com/legal/us-supreme-court-rebuffs-lawyers-punished-after-woeful-suit-backing-trump-2023-10-02/

Shahid F, Vashistha A (2023) Decolonizing Content Moderation: Does Uniform Global Community Standard Resemble Utopian Equality or Western Power Hegemony? Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1145/3544548.3581538

Simonov A, Sacher S, Dubé J-P, Biswas S (2020) The persuasive effect of Fox News: Non-compliance with social distancing during the Covid-19 pandemic (tech. rep.). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27237

Simonov A, Sacher S, Dubé J-P, Biswas S (2022) Frontiers: the persuasive effect of fox news: noncompliance with social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Mark Sci 41:230–242. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2021.1328

Smith P, Bansal-Travers M, O’Connor R, Brown A, Banthin C, Guardino-Colket S, Cummings K (2011) Correcting over 50 years of tobacco industry misinformation. Am J Prev Med 40:690–698

Soroka S, Fournier P, Nir L (2019) Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:18888–18892. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1908369116

Stapleton A (2016) No, you can’t vote by text message. CNN. https://edition.cnn.com/2016/11/07/politics/vote-by-text-message-fake-news/index.html

Starbird K, DiResta R, DeButts M (2023) Influence and Improvisation: participatory disinformation during the 2020 US election. Soc Media Soc 9:20563051231177943. https://doi.org/10.1177/20563051231177943

Supran G, Rahmstorf S, Oreskes N (2023) Assessing ExxonMobil’s global warming projections. Science 379(6628):eabk0063. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abk0063

Supran G, Oreskes N (2017) Assessing ExxonMobil’s climate change communications (1977–2014). Environ Res Lett 12:084019. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aa815f

Supran G, Oreskes N (2021) Rhetoric and frame analysis of ExxonMobil’s climate change communications. One Earth. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oneear.2021.04.014

Swire-Thompson B, Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, Berinsky AJ (2020) They might be a liar but they’re my liar: Source evaluation and the prevalence of misinformation. Polit Psychol 41:21–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12586

Takhshid Z (2021) Regulating social media in the global south. Vanderbilt J Entertain Technol Law 24:1–56

Tenove C (2020) Protecting democracy from disinformation: Normative threats and policy responses. Int J Press/Polit 25:517–537. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161220918740

Terkel A, Timm JC, Gregorian D (2023) Here’s what fox news was trying to hide in its dominion lawsuit redactions. NBC News. https://www.nbcnews.com/politics/elections/dominion-releases-previously-redacted-slides-fox-news-lawsuit-rcna77257

U.S. House of Representatives Judiciary Committee. (2023). News release: Jim Jordan on why the select subcommittee on the weaponization of the federal government is necessary | House Judiciary Committee Republicans. http://judiciary.house.gov/media/press-releases/jim-jordan-on-why-the-select-subcommittee-on-the-weaponization-of-the-federal

Uscinski JE, Parent JM (2014) American conspiracy theories. Oxford University Press

Uscinski JE (2015) The epistemology of fact checking (is still naìve): Rejoinder to Amazeen. Crit Rev 27:243–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/08913811.2015.1055892

Van Der Zee S, Poppe R, Havrileck A, Baillon A (2021) A personal model of trumpery: linguistic deception detection in a real-world high-stakes setting. Psychol Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976211015941

van Doorn M (2023) Advancing the debate on the consequences of misinformation: clarifying why it’s not (just) about false beliefs. Inquiry 0:1–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2023.2289137

Vese D (2022) Governing fake news: the regulation of social media and the right to freedom of expression in the era of emergency. Eur J Risk Regul 13:477–513. https://doi.org/10.1017/err.2021.48

Vosoughi S, Roy D, Aral S (2018) The spread of true and false news online. Science 359:1146–1151. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aap9559

Walasek L, Brown GDA (2023) Incomparability and incommensurability in choice: No common currency of value? Perspect Psychol Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231192828

Wallace J, Goldsmith-Pinkham P, Schwartz JL (2023) Excess death rates for republican and democratic registered voters in Florida and Ohio during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Internal Med. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2023.1154

Wanless A, Berk M (2019) The audience is the amplifier: Participatory propaganda. In P Baines, N O’Shaughnessy, & N Snow (Eds.), The sage handbook of propaganda (pp. 85–104). Sage

Wardle C, Derakhshan H (2017) Information disorder: Toward an interdisciplinary framework for research and policymaking (tech. rep.). Council of Europe. https://rm.coe.int/information-disorder-report-version-august-2018/16808c9c77

Weinschenk AC, Panagopoulos C, van der Linden S (2021) Democratic norms, social projection, and false consensus in the 2020 U.S. presidential election. J Polit Mark 20:255–268. https://doi.org/10.1080/15377857.2021.1939568

West D (2023) We shouldn’t turn disinformation into a constitutional right. Brookings Institution. https://www.brookings.edu/articles/we-shouldnt-turn-disinformation-into-a-constitutional-right/

Williams D (2021) Motivated ignorance, rationality, and democratic politics. Synthese 198:7807–7827. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02549-8

Williams D (2022) The marketplace of rationalizations. Econ Philosophy, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0266267121000389

Wu T (2017) The attention merchants. Atlantic Books

Yee AK (2023a) Information deprivation and democratic engagement. Philos Sci 90:1110–1119. https://doi.org/10.1017/psa.2023.9

Yee AK (2023b) Machine learning, misinformation, and citizen science. Eur J Philosophy Sci 13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-023-00558-1

Zakrzewski C, Lima C, Harwell D (2023) What the Jan. 6 probe found out about social media, but didn’t report. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/technology/2023/01/17/jan6-committee-report-social-media/

Download references

Acknowledgements

SL acknowledges financial support from the European Research Council (ERC Advanced Grant 101020961 PRODEMINFO), the Humboldt Foundation through a research award, the Volkswagen Foundation (grant “Reclaiming individual autonomy and democratic discourse online: How to rebalance human and algorithmic decision making”), and the European Commission (Horizon 2020 grants 964728 JITSUVAX and 101094752 SoMe4Dem). SL also receives funding from Jigsaw (a technology incubator created by Google) and from UK Research and Innovation through EU Horizon replacement funding grant number 10049415. UKHE acknowledges support from the Australian Research Council (grant FT190100708). For the purpose of open access, the author(s) has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Bristol, Bristol, UK

Stephan Lewandowsky

University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia

Ullrich K. H. Ecker

University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Sander van der Linden

Kings College London, London, UK

Jon Roozenbeek

Harvard University, Cambridge, UK

Naomi Oreskes

Boston University, Boston, MA, USA

Lee C. McIntyre

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

The first author created the first draft and all other authors contributed additional material and comments and suggestions and participated jointly in the editing and revision process.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephan Lewandowsky .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

SL, JR, and SvdL have received funding from Google Jigsaw for empirical work on inoculation against misinformation and continue to collaborate with Jigsaw. NO has received funding from the Rockefeller Family Fund to support research on fossil fuel industry disinformation. She has also served as a consultant to the law firm Sher-Edling, who are representing several counties in California suing the fossil fuel industry, and as an expert witness in the defamation case of climate scientist Michael Mann. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors that would require consent.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U.K.H., Cook, J. et al. Liars know they are lying: differentiating disinformation from disagreement. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 11 , 986 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03503-6

Download citation

Received : 25 January 2024

Accepted : 22 July 2024

Published : 31 July 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03503-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

research methods for social sciences

Journal of Methodology of Social Sciences and Humanities; No. 101

Description.

Study of the relationship between philosophy, philosophy of social sciences and humanities, methodology and research method; The role of philosophy and methodology of social sciences and humanities in the quantitative and qualitative improvement of the social sciences and humanities; Comparative study of philosophy and methodology of social sciences and humanities and natural sciences; Study of the evolution of philosophy and methodology of social sciences and humanities; Comparative examination of quantitative and qualitative research methods; Philosophical analysis of concepts used in the social sciences and humanities; The study of the philosophical and theological foundations of the production of religious and indigenous social sciences and humanities; Methodology of the production of religious and indigenous social sciences and humanities; Methodology of using religious texts in social sciences and humanities and social sciences; Methodology of Muslim scholars in social studies; The deficiencies of the social sciences and humanities in the field of goals, foundations, methods and achievements; Philosophical and methodological assessment of studies in the field of humanities and social sciences; Philosophical and methodological assessment of studies in the field of Islamic humanities and social sciences; Philosophical and methodological assessment of macro policies of the country in the scientific, economic, political, social and cultural fields

Institutions

Related links.

medRxiv

Unlocking the transformative potential of data science in improving maternal, newborn and child health in Africa: A scoping review protocol

  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Akuze Joseph Waiswa
  • For correspondence: [email protected]
  • ORCID record for Bancy Ngatia
  • ORCID record for Samson Yahannes Amare
  • ORCID record for Phillip Wanduru
  • ORCID record for Grieven P Otieno
  • ORCID record for Rornald Muhumuza Kananura
  • ORCID record for Fati Kirakoya-Samadoulougou
  • ORCID record for Abiy Seifu Estifanos
  • ORCID record for Eric O Ohuma
  • Info/History
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF

ABSTRACT Introduction: Application of data science in Maternal, Newborn, and Child Health (MNCH) across Africa is variable with limited documentation. Despite efforts to reduce preventable MNCH morbidity and mortality, progress remains slow. Accurate data is crucial for holding countries accountable, tracking progress towards realisation of SDG3 targets on MNCH, and guiding interventions. Data science can improve data availability, quality, healthcare provision, and decision-making for MNCH programs. We aim to map and synthesise use cases of data science in MNCH across Africa. Methods and Analysis: We will develop a conceptual framework encompassing seven domains: Infrastructure and Systemic Challenges, Data Acquisition, Data Quality, Governance, Regulatory Dynamics and Policy, Technological Innovations and Digital Health, Capacity Development, Human Capital, Collaborative and Strategic Frameworks, data analysis, visualization, dissemination and Recommendations for Implementation and Scaling. A scoping review methodology will be used including literature searches in seven databases, grey literature sources and data extraction from the Digital Health Initiatives database. Three reviewers will screen articles and extract data. We will synthesise and present data narratively, and use tables, figures, and maps. Our structured search strategy across academic databases and grey literature sources will find relevant studies on data science in MNCH in Africa. Ethics and dissemination: This scoping review require no formal ethics, because no primary data will be collected. Findings will showcase gaps, opportunities, advances, innovations, implementation, areas needing additional research and propose next steps for integration of data science in MNCH programs in Africa. The findings' implications will be examined in relation to possible methods for enhancing data science in MNCH settings, such as community, and clinical settings, monitoring and evaluation. This study will illuminate data science applications in addressing MNCH issues and provide a holistic view of areas where gaps exist and where there are opportunities to leverage and tap into what already exists. The work will be relevant for stakeholders, policymakers, and researchers in the MNCH field to inform planning. Findings will be disseminated through peer-reviewed journals, conferences, policy briefs, blogs, and social media platforms in Africa. Keywords: Data Science, Maternal Health, Newborn and Perinatal Health, Child Health, Africa

Competing Interest Statement

The authors have declared no competing interest.

Clinical Protocols

https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/8PYNC

Funding Statement

This study did not receive any funding

Author Declarations

I confirm all relevant ethical guidelines have been followed, and any necessary IRB and/or ethics committee approvals have been obtained.

I confirm that all necessary patient/participant consent has been obtained and the appropriate institutional forms have been archived, and that any patient/participant/sample identifiers included were not known to anyone (e.g., hospital staff, patients or participants themselves) outside the research group so cannot be used to identify individuals.

I understand that all clinical trials and any other prospective interventional studies must be registered with an ICMJE-approved registry, such as ClinicalTrials.gov. I confirm that any such study reported in the manuscript has been registered and the trial registration ID is provided (note: if posting a prospective study registered retrospectively, please provide a statement in the trial ID field explaining why the study was not registered in advance).

I have followed all appropriate research reporting guidelines, such as any relevant EQUATOR Network research reporting checklist(s) and other pertinent material, if applicable.

Data Availability

No underlying data is associated with this article

View the discussion thread.

Supplementary Material

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about medRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Twitter logo

Citation Manager Formats

  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Epidemiology
  • Addiction Medicine (336)
  • Allergy and Immunology (658)
  • Anesthesia (177)
  • Cardiovascular Medicine (2567)
  • Dentistry and Oral Medicine (310)
  • Dermatology (218)
  • Emergency Medicine (390)
  • Endocrinology (including Diabetes Mellitus and Metabolic Disease) (910)
  • Epidemiology (12075)
  • Forensic Medicine (10)
  • Gastroenterology (741)
  • Genetic and Genomic Medicine (3986)
  • Geriatric Medicine (375)
  • Health Economics (666)
  • Health Informatics (2574)
  • Health Policy (992)
  • Health Systems and Quality Improvement (956)
  • Hematology (357)
  • HIV/AIDS (824)
  • Infectious Diseases (except HIV/AIDS) (13568)
  • Intensive Care and Critical Care Medicine (783)
  • Medical Education (394)
  • Medical Ethics (106)
  • Nephrology (422)
  • Neurology (3750)
  • Nursing (206)
  • Nutrition (559)
  • Obstetrics and Gynecology (717)
  • Occupational and Environmental Health (686)
  • Oncology (1953)
  • Ophthalmology (565)
  • Orthopedics (233)
  • Otolaryngology (300)
  • Pain Medicine (247)
  • Palliative Medicine (72)
  • Pathology (469)
  • Pediatrics (1088)
  • Pharmacology and Therapeutics (453)
  • Primary Care Research (442)
  • Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology (3347)
  • Public and Global Health (6419)
  • Radiology and Imaging (1357)
  • Rehabilitation Medicine and Physical Therapy (793)
  • Respiratory Medicine (857)
  • Rheumatology (394)
  • Sexual and Reproductive Health (396)
  • Sports Medicine (336)
  • Surgery (431)
  • Toxicology (51)
  • Transplantation (184)
  • Urology (162)

IMAGES

  1. Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction

    research methods for social sciences

  2. Methodology in Social Science Research

    research methods for social sciences

  3. Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices by Anol

    research methods for social sciences

  4. Research Methods in Sociology

    research methods for social sciences

  5. Social Science Research

    research methods for social sciences

  6. Research Methodology for Social Sciences

    research methods for social sciences

VIDEO

  1. Research Methods & Social Sciences

  2. Introduction Blues: How to Hook Your Readers from the Start #irfannawaz #research #phd #tips

  3. Making APA table in Microsoft Word of independent sample t test

  4. If a friend was thinking of studying at Cardiff University, I would say

  5. My favourite thing about the School of Social Sciences is

  6. Exploring Research Methodologies in the Social Sciences (4 Minutes)

COMMENTS

  1. Research Methods for the Social Sciences (Pelz)

    It is a one-stop, comprehensive, and compact source for foundational concepts in behavioral research, and can serve as a stand-alone text or as a supplement to research readings in any doctoral seminar or research methods class. This text will introduce you to the fascinating and important study of the methods of inquiry in the Social Sciences.

  2. Social Science Research: Principles, Methods and Practices

    This book is designed to introduce doctoral and postgraduate students to the process of conducting scientific research in the social sciences, business, education, public health, and related disciplines. It is a one-stop, comprehensive, and compact source for foundational concepts in behavioural research, and can serve as a standalone text or as a supplement to research readings in any ...

  3. Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction

    Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction. Valerie Sheppard. Download this book. This textbook provides a broad overview of research methods utilized in sociology. It will be of particular value for students who are new to research methods. Creative Commons Attribution NonCommercial ShareAlike. Read Book.

  4. Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction

    Features of Research Methods for the Social Sciences The following briefly describes the special features of this text. Chapters This textbook is organized as a collection of chapters that can be rearranged and modified to suit the needs of a particular faculty or class. That

  5. Research Methods for the Social Sciences: An Introduction

    About This Book. Chapter 1: Introduction to Research Methods. Chapter 2: Ethics in Research. Chapter 3: Developing a Research Question. Chapter 4: Measurement and Units of Analysis. Chapter 5: The Literature Review. Chapter 6: Data Collection Strategies. Chapter 7: Sampling Techniques. Chapter 8: Data Collection Methods: Survey Research.

  6. Research Methods

    Sage Research Methods Online (SRMO). SRMO provides access to information about research methods compiled from a variety of Sage publications, including books/handbooks, articles, and the "Little Green Book" series, Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences.SRMO is searchable and browsable by author, and it includes a methods map, as well as video tutorials.

  7. Research Methods in the Social Sciences: an A-Z of Key Concepts

    Research Methods in the Social Sciences is a comprehensive yet compact A-Z for undergraduate and postgraduate students undertaking research across the social sciences, featuring 71 entries that cover a wide range of concepts, methods, and theories. Each entry begins with an accessible introduction to a method, using real-world examples from a wide range of academic disciplines, before ...

  8. (PDF) Research-Methods-for-the-Social-Sciences-An-Introduction Updated

    Abstract and Figures. This text is written for undergraduate social science students. It is intended to be used in a one or two-semester introductory research methods course. NEyē ʔ Sqȃ ...

  9. Research Methods in the Social Sciences: An A-Z of key concepts

    Research Methods in the Social Sciences features chapters that cover a wide range of concepts, methods, and theories. Each chapter begins with an introduction to a method, using real-world examples from a wide range of academic disciplines, before discussing the benefits and limitations of the approach, its current status in academic practice, and finally providing tips and advice on when and ...

  10. The SAGE Encyclopedia of Social Science Research Methods

    In addition to epistemological issues that influence the nature of research questions and assumptions, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Social Science Research Methods tackles topics not normally viewed as part of social science research methodology, from philosophical issues such as poststructuralism to advanced statistical techniques. Quantitative ...

  11. Sage Research Methods

    Qualitative Research for the Social Sciences is an interdisciplinary core text on introductory qualitative research for social science disciplines. With a focus on the integral role of the researcher, Marilyn Lichtman uses a conversational writing style that draws readers into the excitement of the research process.

  12. Organizing Your Social Sciences Research Paper

    I. Groups of Research Methods. There are two main groups of research methods in the social sciences: The empirical-analytical group approaches the study of social sciences in a similar manner that researchers study the natural sciences.This type of research focuses on objective knowledge, research questions that can be answered yes or no, and operational definitions of variables to be measured.

  13. Research Methods for the Social Sciences

    Preface. Chapter 1 Science and Scientific Research. Chapter 2 Thinking Like a Researcher. Chapter 3 The Research Process. Chapter 4 Theories in Scientific Research. Chapter 5 Research Design. Chapter 6 Measurement of Constructs. Chapter 7 Scale Reliability and Validity. Chapter 8 Sampling.

  14. PDF Social Science Methodology

    approaches to analysis, as well as relevant issues in philosophy of social science. Forthcoming Titles Michael Coppedge, Approaching Democracy: Theory and Methods in Comparative Politics Thad Dunning, Natural Experiments in the Social Sciences Diana Kapiszewski, Lauren M. MacLean and Benjamin L. Read, Field Research in Political Science

  15. PDF Qualitative Research Methods for the Social Sciences

    Qualitative Research Methods for the Social Sciences Howard Lune Hunter College, CUNY Bruce L. Berg California State University, Long Beach A01_LUNE2136_09_SE_FM.indd 1 6/6/16 7:55 PM NOT ... Subjects: LCSH: Social sciences—Research—Methodology. Classification: LCC H61 .B4715 2016 | DDC 300.72/1—dc23 LC record available at https://lccn ...

  16. Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices

    Bhattacherjee, Anol, "Social Science Research: Principles, Methods, and Practices" (2012). Textbooks Collection. 3. This book is designed to introduce doctoral and graduate students to the process of scientific research in the social sciences, business, education, public health, and related disciplines. It is a one-stop, comprehensive, and ...

  17. Social Sciences Research Methods

    The following sources provide explanations, examples and procedures about various methodologies used in the social sciences research. (SRM) is the major tool for exploring various methodologies. SRM includes the Little Green and Little Blue book series, articles, case studies, databases and videos covering quantitative, qualitative, and mixed ...

  18. (PDF) Qualitative Research Approaches in Social Sciences

    This article presents a comprehensive exploration of commonly utilized qualitative research methods in the social sciences. Covering essential methodologies such as ethnography, phenomenology ...

  19. The SAGE Handbook of Social Research Methods

    Preview. The SAGE Handbook of Social Research Methods is a must for every social-science researcher. It charts the new and evolving terrain of social research methodology, covering qualitative, quantitative and mixed methods in one volume. The Handbook includes chapters on each phase of the research process: research design, methods of data ...

  20. Research Methods in the Social Sciences: An A-Z of key concepts

    Research Methods in the Social Sciences is a comprehensive yet compact A-Z for undergraduate and postgraduate students undertaking qualitative and quantitative research across the social sciences, featuring 71 entries that cover a wide range of concepts, methods, and theories.

  21. Introduction to qualitative research methods

    Qualitative research methods are widely used in the social sciences and the humanities, but they can also complement quantitative approaches used in clinical research. ... Second, qualitative research methods equip us in studying complex phenomena. Qualitative research methods provide scientific tools for exploring and identifying the numerous ...

  22. Research Methods in the Social Sciences

    Chava Frankfort-Nachmias is an Emeritus Professor of Sociology at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee. In addition to Research Methods in the Social Sciences, she is coauthor of Social Statistics for a Diverse Society, coeditor of Sappho in the Holy Land (with Erella Shadmi) and numerous publications on ethnicity and development, urban revitalization, science and gender, and women in Israel.

  23. What Is Quantitative Research? An Overview and Guidelines

    The necessity, importance, relevance, and urgency of quantitative research are articulated, establishing a strong foundation for the subsequent discussion, which delineates the scope, objectivity, goals, data, and methods that distinguish quantitative research, alongside a balanced inspection of its strengths and shortcomings, particularly in ...

  24. Social Science Methodology

    It offers a one-volume introduction to social science methodology relevant to the disciplines of anthropology, economics, history, political science, psychology and sociology. This new edition has been extensively developed with the introduction of new material and a thorough treatment of essential elements such as conceptualization ...

  25. Liars know they are lying: differentiating disinformation from ...

    Mis- and disinformation pose substantial societal challenges, and have thus become the focus of a substantive field of research. However, the field of misinformation research has recently come ...

  26. Self-affirmation increases reemployment success for the unemployed

    PNAS is a prestigious journal that publishes cutting-edge research in various fields of natural science. In this article, you can find out how a mathematical model can explain the evolution of cooperation and extortion in the Prisoner's Dilemma, a classic game theory problem. Read more to discover the insights and implications of this study.

  27. Journal of Methodology of Social Sciences and Humanities; No. 101

    Description. Study of the relationship between philosophy, philosophy of social sciences and humanities, methodology and research method; The role of philosophy and methodology of social sciences and humanities in the quantitative and qualitative improvement of the social sciences and humanities; Comparative study of philosophy and methodology of social sciences and humanities and natural ...

  28. Unlocking the transformative potential of data science in improving

    The findings' implications will be examined in relation to possible methods for enhancing data science in MNCH settings, such as community, and clinical settings, monitoring and evaluation. This study will illuminate data science applications in addressing MNCH issues and provide a holistic view of areas where gaps exist and where there are ...